Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Brain Region–Specific Decrease in the Activity and Expression of Protein Kinase A in the Frontal Cortex of Regressive Autism

  • Lina Ji,

    Affiliations NYS Institute for Basic Research in Developmental Disabilities, Staten Island, New York, United States of America, The State Key Lab of Pharmaceutical Biotechnology, College of Life Sciences, Nanjing University, Nanjing, People's Republic of China

  • Ved Chauhan,

    Affiliation NYS Institute for Basic Research in Developmental Disabilities, Staten Island, New York, United States of America

  • Michael J. Flory,

    Affiliation NYS Institute for Basic Research in Developmental Disabilities, Staten Island, New York, United States of America

  • Abha Chauhan

    abha.chauhan@opwdd.ny.gov

    Affiliation NYS Institute for Basic Research in Developmental Disabilities, Staten Island, New York, United States of America

Abstract

Autism is a severe neurodevelopmental disorder that is characterized by impaired language, communication, and social skills. In regressive autism, affected children first show signs of normal social and language development but eventually lose these skills and develop autistic behavior. Protein kinases are essential in G-protein-coupled, receptor-mediated signal transduction and are involved in neuronal functions, gene expression, memory, and cell differentiation. We studied the activity and expression of protein kinase A (PKA), a cyclic AMP–dependent protein kinase, in postmortem brain tissue samples from the frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital cortices, and the cerebellum of individuals with regressive autism; autistic subjects without a clinical history of regression; and age-matched developmentally normal control subjects. The activity of PKA and the expression of PKA (C-α), a catalytic subunit of PKA, were significantly decreased in the frontal cortex of individuals with regressive autism compared to control subjects and individuals with non-regressive autism. Such changes were not observed in the cerebellum, or the cortices from the temporal, parietal, and occipital regions of the brain in subjects with regressive autism. In addition, there was no significant difference in PKA activity or expression of PKA (C-α) between non-regressive autism and control groups. These results suggest that regression in autism may be associated, in part, with decreased PKA-mediated phosphorylation of proteins and abnormalities in cellular signaling.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are neurodevelopmental disorders characterized by impairment in social interactions and verbal/non-verbal communication skills, and restricted, repetitive and stereotyped patterns of behavior [1]. According to a recent report from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, the prevalence of ASDs is 1 in 110 for children 8 years of age [2]. The symptoms of ASDs are typically present before the age of 3 years, and are often accompanied by abnormalities in cognitive functioning, learning, attention, and sensory processing. While the causes of ASDs remain elusive, ASDs are considered to be heterogeneous and multifactorial disorders that are influenced by both genetic and environmental factors. The onset of autism is gradual in many children. However, in regressive autism, children first show signs of normal social and language development but lose these developmental skills at 15–24 months and develop autistic behavior [3]. The reported incidence of regressive autism varies in different studies from 15% to 62% of cases [4][7]. In a few cases, regression may significantly affect language, with lesser impact in other domains such as social interaction or imaginative play [4], [8]. On the other hand, some children may regress especially in social functions and not in language [9].

Protein kinases are known to play important roles in cellular signaling pathways and are involved in brain development [10][13]. Protein kinase A (PKA) is a cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP)–dependent protein kinase that is involved in cognitive functions and memory formation [14][18]. PKA consists of regulatory (R) and catalytic (C) subunits. Three genes encode for catalytic units (Cα, Cβ, and Cγ), and four other genes encode for regulatory units (RIα, RIβ, RIIα, and RIIβ) of PKA. PKA remains catalytically inactive when the levels of cAMP are low. The concentration of cAMP rises upon activation of adenylate cyclase by G protein-coupled receptors, and/or inhibition of cyclic nucleotide phosphodiesterase (PDE) enzyme. Under these conditions, cAMP binds to two binding sites on the regulatory subunits of PKA, which results in the release of the catalytic subunits. These free catalytic units of PKA can then phosphorylate proteins by transferring a phosphate group from ATP. Several studies have implicated the role of PKA in neuropsychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia, bipolar affective disorder, obsessive compulsive disorder, and panic disorders [19][22]. To date, no studies of PKA have been done in autism.

The intracellular levels of cAMP are controlled by PDE, which degrades the phosphodiester bond in cAMP. PDE regulates the localization, duration, and amplitude of cAMP signaling within subcellular domains. Multiple forms of PDEs have been identified on the basis of substrate specificity. PDE4, 7, and 8 act on cAMP; PDE5, 6, and 9 act on cyclic guanosine monophosphate (cGMP); whereas PDE1, 2, 3, 10, and 11 act on both cAMP and cGMP. Recent evidence has suggested altered levels of PDE4 in the brains of individuals with autism [23].

Because the levels of PDE4 are altered in autism, and PKA is involved in neuropsychiatric disorders, it was of interest to compare the activity and protein levels of PKA in different brain regions in autism (regressive and non-regressive) and age-matched control subjects. Our study suggests that PKA activity and expression are decreased in the frontal cortex of individuals with regressive autism as compared with control subjects. Such changes were not observed in individuals with non-regressive autism.

Materials and Methods

Autism and Control Subjects

Samples of postmortem frozen brain regions, i.e., the cerebellum, and the cortices from the frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital lobes from autistic (N = 7–10 for different brain regions) and age-matched, typically developed, control subjects (N = 9–10) were obtained from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Brain and Tissue Bank for Developmental Disorders at the University of Maryland, Baltimore, MD. The age (mean ± S.E.) for autistic subjects was 12.6±3.2 years, and for control subjects, 12.4±3.3 years. All brain samples were stored at −70°C.

The case history and clinical characteristics for the autism and control subjects are summarized in Table 1. Donors with autism had met the diagnostic criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual-IV for autism. The Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) test was performed for the donors UMB #s 4671, 4849, 1174, 797, 1182, 4899, and 1638 (Table 2). Each donor's impairments in social interaction, qualitative abnormalities in communication, and restricted, repetitive and stereotyped patterns of behavior are consistent with the diagnosis of autism, according to the results of the ADI-R diagnostic algorithm. All donors with autism exceeded the cut-off score in these parameters. The diagnosis of autism was assigned to donor UMB # 1349 after extensive evaluation of behavioral tests, including the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS), Vineland Adaptive Behavioral Scale (VABS), and Bayley Scales for Infant Development-II (BSID-II). In addition to the ADI-R, UMB # 4849 was also evaluated by the BSID-II and Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS), which indicated moderate to severe autism, and autism in UMB # 4671 was also verified by the VABS and BSID-II. Table 3 shows scores for the VABS test, which assesses adaptive behavior in four domains: communication, daily living skills, socialization, and motor skills.

thumbnail
Table 1. Case history and clinical characteristics of autism and control donors of brain tissue samples.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023751.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised test scores in donors of brain tissue samples.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023751.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Vineland Adaptive Behavioral Scales diagnostic test for autism in donors of brain tissue samples.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023751.t003

In this study, the subjects with autism were divided into two subgroups: regressive autism and non-regressive autism, depending on the pattern of onset of symptoms for autism. Regressive autism is a type of autism in which early development is normal, followed by a loss of previously acquired skills. Language is the most common area that regresses; this regression can be accompanied by more global regression involving loss of social skills and social interest. On the other hand, in non-regressive autism, the child never gains normal language and social skills, and initial symptoms are delayed speech development, and/or delay in development of social skills and in nonverbal communication. These children do not demonstrate regression in terms of loss of language or social skills.

Ethics statement.

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the New York State Institute for Basic Research in Developmental Disabilities. The IRB reviewed this study in accordance with New York State Regulations and the HHS Office for Human Research Protections, including the “Human Subject Decision Chart 1,” and found that the research does not involve human subjects because “the research does not involve intervention or interaction with the individuals”, nor “is the information individually identifiable”. The subjects cannot be identified, directly or through identifiers linked to the system, and the consent is not required.

Preparation of Brain Homogenates

The tissue samples were homogenized (10% w/v) in cold buffer containing 50 mM Tris-HCl (pH 7.4), 8.5% sucrose, 2 mM EDTA, 10 mM β-mercaptoethanol, and protease inhibitor cocktail (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) at 4°C. For extraction of protein kinases, the homogenates were mixed with an equal volume of extraction buffer containing 40 mM Tris-HCl (pH 7.4), 300 mM NaCl, 2 mM EDTA, 2 mM EGTA, 2% Triton, 5 mM sodium pyrophosphate, 2 mM β-glycerophosphate, 2 mM Na3VO4, 100 mM NaF, and 2 µg/ml leupeptin. The samples were allowed to stand on ice for 10 min, and then centrifuged at 135,000 g for 20 min at 4°C. The supernatants were collected, and the concentrations of total proteins in the supernatants were measured by the biocinchoninic acid protein assay kit (Thermo Scientific, Rockford, IL).

Assay for PKA Activity

PKA activity was measured using the solid phase enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) kit from Enzo Life Sciences International, Inc. (Plymouth Meeting, PA). In this assay, the substrate of PKA was pre-coated on the wells of a microplate. The microplate wells were soaked with 50 µl of kinase assay dilution buffer for 10 min. The buffer was then carefully aspirated from each well, and the brain samples were added to the appropriate wells. The kinase reaction was initiated by adding 10 µl ATP, and was carried out for 90 min at 30°C. It was terminated by emptying the contents of each well. A phosphosubstrate–specific antibody was added to the wells except in blank, and incubated for 60 min at room temperature, followed by washing 4 times with wash buffer. The peroxidase-conjugated secondary antibody was then added except in blank, and incubation was done for 60 min at room temperature. The wells were again washed 4 times with wash buffer. The color was developed with tetramethylbenzidine substrate, and it was proportional to the phosphotransferase activity of PKA. The reaction was stopped with acid-stop solution, and the absorbance was measured at 450 nm in a microplate reader. The absorbance was divided by the concentration of total protein (µg) in each sample, and the data are represented as relative PKA activity.

Western Blot Analysis

Total protein (15 µg) from brain homogenates of subjects with regressive- and non-regressive autism or control subjects was separated using a 10% sodium dodecyl sulfate-polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis, and then transferred to a nitrocellulose membrane. The membrane was blocked with Tris-buffered saline containing 5% fat-free dried milk for 2 h at room temperature, and further incubated overnight at 4°C with polyclonal antibody against C-subunit (isoform α) of PKA (Cell Signaling Technology Inc., Danvers, MA). The membrane was then washed 3 times with TBS-0.05% Tween 20, and incubated with horseradish peroxidase-conjugated secondary antibody for 2 h at room temperature. The membrane was washed again, and the immunoreactive protein was visualized using enhanced chemiluminescent reagent. Because PKA (C-α) and β-actin have similar molecular weights (42 KDa), polyclonal antibody against PKA (C-α) was stripped from nitrocellulose membrane, and the membrane was re-probed with monoclonal antibody against β-actin (loading control). The densities of all protein bands were measured by NIH Image J software, and the density of PKA (C-α) band was normalized with the density of β-actin for each sample.

Statistical Analysis

Initially, autistic and control cases were collected as age-matched pairs. As data for both members of a pair were not available in all cases, and data were approximately normally distributed, unpaired two-tailed t-tests were employed to make comparisons of PKA activity in various brain regions, and of overall PKA density between autistic vs. control cases. Comparisons among controls and autistic cases showing or not showing clinical signs of regression in function were made using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). To guard against type I error, a Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons was made to the t-tests of multiple brain regions, and for the pairwise post-hoc t-tests comparing each pair of the three groups that were compared in the overall ANOVA. For purposes of this adjustment, tests of different hypotheses, i.e., of activity levels and of protein contents of PKA, were not considered to be multiple comparisons.

Results

PKA Activity in Different Brain Regions of Individuals with Autism and Age-Matched Control Subjects: Relationship with Regression in Autism

The activity of PKA was measured in the brain homogenates from the frontal, temporal, occipital, and parietal cortices, and the cerebellum in autistic and control subjects (Fig. 1). When all autism cases (regressive and non-regressive) were compared with the age-matched control group, no significant difference was found in PKA activity in any of these brain regions, although PKA activity in the frontal cortex was found to be reduced by 34.7% in the autism vs. control group. When the autism group was divided into two sub-groups (regressive and non-regressive), depending on whether there was a clinical history of regression or not, unadjusted two-tailed t-test showed a significant decrease in PKA activity in the frontal cortex of individuals with regressive autism as compared to the developmentally normal control group (p = 0.0278) and the non-regressive autism group (p = 0.0318), but these differences did not remain significant after application of the adjustment for multiple comparisons. The mean ± S.E. of PKA activity in the frontal cortex was: 2.48±0.57 in autism (regressive+non-regressive), 1.60±0.31 in regressive autism, 3.94±0.99 in non-regressive autism, and 3.80±0.65 in control groups. The alteration in PKA activity was specific to the frontal cortex in regressive autism because it was not observed in other regions of the brain, i.e., the cerebellum and the temporal, parietal, and occipital cortices, suggesting that the changes observed in PKA activity were brain region– specific in regressive autism. PKA activity was also similar in all of the brain regions between non-regressive autism and control groups.

thumbnail
Figure 1. PKA activity in different brain regions from regressive autism, non-regressive autism, and age-matched control subjects.

The autism group comprises combined regressive and non-regressive autism sub-groups. Brain homogenates were prepared, and activity of PKA was measured as described in Materials and Methods. Data represent mean ± S.E.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023751.g001

There was no significant difference in postmortem interval (PMI) between the autistic and control groups, or between the regressive autism and non-regressive autism groups. The mean ± S.E. of PMI was: 22.0±4.2 in the autism groups (regressive+non-regressive, n = 10), 16.1±1.22 in the control group (n = 10), 28.4±7.2 in regressive autism (n = 5), and 15.6±2.6 in the non-regressive autism group (n = 5). We also studied whether there was an inverse correlation between PMI and PKA activity. Correlation analysis between PMI and PKA activity for all autistic and control subjects did not reveal any such association (data not shown). Furthermore, the cerebellum and the temporal, parietal, and occipital cortices were not affected in subjects with regressive autism in comparison with control subjects, while the frontal cortex was affected in these individuals. These results suggest that PMI was not a contributing factor to the observed alteration in PKA activity in the frontal cortex of individuals with regressive autism. There was also no significant difference in age (mean ± S.E.) between the regressive autism (11.6±2.7 years, n = 5) and non-regressive autism groups (13.7±6.1 years, n = 5).

Protein Levels of Catalytic C-α Subunit of PKA in the Frontal Cortex of Individuals with Autism (Regressive and Non-Regressive) and Control Subjects

Because a decrease in PKA activity was observed in the frontal cortex of subjects with regressive autism as compared to control subjects and subjects with non-regressive autism, we analyzed whether the decreased activity of PKA is related to the reduced protein contents of PKA. The protein contents of the catalytic Cα unit of PKA were analyzed in the frontal cortex of individuals with autism (regressive and non-regressive) and age-matched controls by Western blotting (Fig. 2 A). The relative densities of the protein contents of PKA (C-α) normalized with β-actin are shown in Fig. 2 B. A one-way ANOVA comparing regressive and non-regressive autism cases and controls showed a significant difference in the protein contents among these three groups (F [df = 2,15] = 9.770, p = 0.002). Post-hoc pairwise comparisons among the groups revealed a significant decrease in the protein contents of PKA (C-α) in individuals with regressive autism (mean ± S.E = 0.34±0.09) as compared to control (mean ± S.E. = 0.64±0.05, p = 0.019, Bonferroni-adjusted) and individuals with non-regressive autism (mean ± S.E. = 0.83±0.09, p = 0.002, Bonferroni-adjusted), suggesting that the protein contents of PKA are affected in regressive autism. PKA contents were similar between non-regressive autism and control groups, and when the entire autism group (regressive and non-regressive) was compared with the control group.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Relative protein levels of PKA (C-α) in the frontal cortex of regressive autism, non-regressive autism, and age-matched control subjects.

Western blot analyses of C-α subunit of PKA in the frontal cortex of individuals with regressive and non-regressive autism, and age-matched control subjects are represented in Fig. 2A. The relative density of PKA (C-α) normalized with the density of β-actin (loading control) is shown in Fig. 2B. Data represent mean ± S.E.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023751.g002

Discussion

ASDs are complex neurodevelopmental disorders. The complexity of ASDs is further increased because some affected individuals fall in the sub-group of regressive autism [7]. Behavioral changes in regressive autism fall into two broad domains: (a) loss of vocalization and (b) loss of social skills. The rate of regressive autism varies from 15% to 62% of cases in different studies [4][7]. While Lord et al. reported that 29% of the children they studied who were diagnosed with autism had lost language skills for meaningful words, and another 9% lost non-word vocalizations [5], Goldberg et al. reported regression in 62% of children [4]. Loss of spoken words generally associates with loss of social behavior [6], but some affected children show only loss of social skills [4]. We report here that individuals with regressive autism have decreased PKA activity in the frontal cortex of the brain. This decreased PKA activity in autistic regression may be attributed to the decreased protein contents of PKA because the protein content of PKA (C-α subunit) was also decreased in the frontal cortex of individuals with regressive autism. Interestingly, such changes were not observed in other brain regions of individuals with regressive autism, or in the frontal cortex and other brain regions of individuals with non-regressive autism. These results suggest that alterations in PKA activity and PKA expression are specific to the frontal lobe in regressive autism.

Our results suggest that PMI and age cannot account for the observed alteration in PKA in regressive autism. Other factors, such as comorbidity with seizure disorder, reported for three of 10 autism cases (of which two had regressive autism, and one had non-regressive autism), and medications, reported for two regressive autism cases, four non-regressive autism cases, and two control cases, do not seem to be contributing factors to the altered activity or expression of PKA in regressive autism. However, further studies with a larger autistic group should be done to explore this issue.

cAMP is one of the key factors for neuronal outgrowth, plasticity, and regeneration. Members of the cAMP-dependent second-messenger pathways participate in the regulation of cellular growth and differentiation and are also implicated in a variety of embryonic stages including brain development [24]. The PKA pathway is also recognized as an essential component in memory formation. Several studies in Drosophila have demonstrated the role of PKA in memory formation [25][29]. Mutations in the rutabaga gene, which encodes adenylate cyclase, caused significant defects in short-term memory [25]. Reduced expression or activity of DC0 (the gene encoding the catalytic subunit of PKA) caused deficits in learning, short-term memory, and middle-term memory [26][28]. Studies have also shown that pharmacological agents such as cAMP analogs and rolipram (an inhibitor of PDE), which are known to increase PKA activity, could improve memory [30], [31].

G-protein–coupled adenylate cyclase converts ATP to cAMP, which in turn binds to regulatory subunits of PKA. Following this event, catalytic subunits of PKA are released, which are the activated forms of PKA. PKA then phosphorylates and alters the activity of enzymes and many target proteins such as ion channels, chromosomal proteins, and transcription factors. cAMP response-binding protein (CREB) is one of the targets of PKA-mediated phosphorylation. CREB, upon activation by PKA, binds to certain DNA sequences (cAMP response elements), thereby stimulating the transcription of downstream genes and the synthesis of proteins. The CREB transcription factor is also required for long-term memory formation [32][34]. It is possible that a decrease in the activity of PKA in regressive autism may result in reduced phosphorylation of CREB, and thus reduced transcription and altered synthesis of some proteins.

Given that PKA is activated by cAMP, and PDE regulates the levels of cAMP, a discussion on PDE becomes imperative. Altered levels of PDE4 in the cerebella of autism subjects were reported by Fatemi and group [23]. Other studies have suggested a role of PDE4 in learning and memory in behavioral models of mice, rats, and monkeys [35], [36]. PDE4 is also reported to be involved in behavior sensitivity to antidepressant drugs in animals [37]. PDE inhibitors such as rolipram could improve object recognition [38], [39], passive avoidance [40], [41], radial arm maze [40][42], Morris water maze [43], and contexual fear conditioning [30], [43], [44]. PDE4 has also been studied as a potential therapeutic target for depressive disorders. It has been suggested that rolipram may have potential therapeutic benefits for major depression [45], Alzheimer's disease [36], [46], Parkinson's disease [47], [48], schizophrenia [49], [50], and tardive dyskinesia [51], [52].

Several reports suggest that some proteins related to the PKA pathway are involved in autism. Extensive evidence indicates hyperserotonemia in autism [53][55]. PKA regulates serotonergic activity in the brain [56]. Galter and Unsicker [57] reported that co-activation of cAMP- and tyrosine receptor kinase B (TrkB)–dependent signaling pathways plays an important role in maintaining the serotonergic neuronal phenotype. TrkB is also regulated by the cAMP/CREB pathway in neurons [58]. Furthermore, transcriptional activity of the engrailed-2 gene is also regulated by PKA [59]. The importance of engrailed can be envisioned because of its crucial roles in brain development [60] and in the development of autism [61][65].

In conclusion, this study suggests that the frontal cortex may be the region of the brain involved in regressive autism, where abnormalities such as decreased activity and expression of PKA can affect the signal transduction. It may have multiple effects on signal transduction pathways, which may also influence serotonergic neurons, TrkB, and engrailed-2, all of which have been suggested to be involved in the development of autism.

Acknowledgments

Human brain tissues were obtained from the NICHD Brain and Tissue Bank for Developmental Disorders at the University of Maryland, Baltimore, MD.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: AC. Performed the experiments: LJ. Analyzed the data: AC VC MJF. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: AC. Wrote the paper: AC VC.

References

  1. 1. Lord C, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, Amaral DG (2000) Autism spectrum disorders. Neuron 28: 355–363.
  2. 2. Rice C (2009) Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders - Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, United States, 2006. MMWR Surveill Summ 58: 1–20.
  3. 3. Ozonoff S, Williams BJ, Landa R (2005) Parental report of the early development of children with regressive autism: the delays-plus-regression phenotype. Autism 9: 461–486.
  4. 4. Goldberg WA, Osann K, Filipek PA, Laulhere T, Jarvis K, et al. (2003) Language and other regression: assessment and timing. J Autism Dev Disord 33: 607–616.
  5. 5. Lord C, Shulman C, DiLavore P (2004) Regression and word loss in autistic spectrum disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 45: 936–955.
  6. 6. Hansen RL, Ozonoff S, Krakowiak P, Angkustsiri K, Jones C, et al. (2008) Regression in autism: prevalence and associated factors in the CHARGE Study. Ambul Pediatr 8: 25–31.
  7. 7. Stefanatos GA (2008) Regression in autistic spectrum disorders. Neuropsychol Rev 18: 305–319.
  8. 8. Stefanatos GA, Grover W, Geller E (1995) Case study: corticosteroid treatment of language regression in pervasive developmental disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 34: 1107–1111.
  9. 9. Luyster R, Richler J, Risi S, Hsu WL, Dawson G, et al. (2005) Early regression in social communication in autism spectrum disorders: a CPEA Study. Dev Neuropsychol 27: 311–336.
  10. 10. Alcazar A, Fando JL, Azuara C, Galea E, Salinas M (1986) Protein kinase activities associated with ribosomes of developing rat brain. Identification of eukaryotic initiation factor 2 kinases. Int J Dev Neurosci 4: 525–535.
  11. 11. Hamada H, Zhang YL, Kawai A, Li F, Hibino Y, et al. (2003) Development-associated myristoylated alanine-rich C kinase substrate phosphorylation in rat brain. Childs Nerv Syst 19: 152–158.
  12. 12. Leonard AS, Hell JW (1997) Cyclic AMP-dependent protein kinase and protein kinase C phosphorylate N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors at different sites. J Biol Chem 272: 12107–12115.
  13. 13. Turner RS, Raynor RL, Mazzei GJ, Girard PR, Kuo JF (1984) Developmental studies of phospholipid-sensitive Ca2+-dependent protein kinase and its substrates and of phosphoprotein phosphatases in rat brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 81: 3143–3147.
  14. 14. Abel T, Nguven PV (2008) Regulation of hippocampus-dependent memory by cyclic AMP-dependent protein kinase. Prog Brain Res 169: 97–115.
  15. 15. Micheau J, Riedel G (1999) Protein kinases: which one is the memory molecule? Cell Mol Life Sci 55: 534–548.
  16. 16. Nie T, McDonough CB, Huang T, Nguyen PV, Abel T (2007) Genetic disruption of protein kinase A anchoring reveals a role for compartmentalized kinase signaling in theta-burst long-term potentiation and spatial memory. J Neurosci 27: 10278–10288.
  17. 17. Sebeo J, Hsiao K, Bozdagi O, Dumitriu D, Ge Y, et al. (2009) Requirement for protein synthesis at developing synapses. J Neurosci 29: 9778–9793.
  18. 18. Nguven PV, Woo NH (2003) Regulation of hippocampal synaptic plasticity by cyclic AMP-dependent protein Kinases. Prog Neurobiol 71: 401–437.
  19. 19. Karege F, Schwald M, Papadimitriou P, Lachausse C, Cisse M (2004) The cAMP-dependent protein kinase A and brain-derived neurotrophic factor expression in lymphoblast cells of bipolar affective disorder. J Affect Disord 79: 187–192.
  20. 20. Tardito D, Tura GB, Bocchio L, Bignotti S, Pioli R, et al. (2000) Abnormal levels of cAMP-dependent protein kinase regulatory subunits in platelets from schizophrenic patients. Neuropsychopharmacology 23: 216–219.
  21. 21. Tardito D, Maina G, Tura GB, Bogetto F, Pioli R, et al. (2001) The cAMP-dependent protein kinase substrate Rap1 in platelets from patients with obsessive compulsive disorder or schizophrenia. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 11: 221–225.
  22. 22. Tardito D, Zanardi R, Racagni G, Manzoni T, Perez J (2002) The protein kinase A in platelets from patients with panic disorder. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 12: 483–487.
  23. 23. Braun NN, Reutiman TJ, Lee S, Folsom TD, Fatemi SH (2007) Expression of phosphodiesterase 4 is altered in the brains of subjects with autism. Neuroreport 18: 1841–1844.
  24. 24. Blaschke RJ, Monaghan AP, Bock D, Rappold GA (2000) A novel murine PKA-related protein kinase involved in neuronal differentiation. Genomics 64: 187–194.
  25. 25. Tully T, Quinn WG (1985) Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutant Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A 157: 263–277.
  26. 26. Goodwin SF, Del Vecchio M, Velinzon K, Hogel C, Russell SR, et al. (1997) Defective learning in mutants of the Drosophila gene for a regulatory subunit of cAMP-dependent protein kinase. J Neurosci 17: 8817–8827.
  27. 27. Li W, Tully T, Kalderon D (1996) Effects of a conditional Drosophila PKA mutant on olfactory learning and memory. Learn Mem 2: 320–333.
  28. 28. Skoulakis EM, Kalderon D, Davis RL (1993) Preferential expression in mushroom bodies of the catalytic subunit of protein kinase A and its role in learning and memory. Neuron 11: 197–208.
  29. 29. Horiuchi J, Yamazaki D, Naganos S, Aigaki T, Saitoe M (2008) Protein kinase A inhibits a consolidated form of memory in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105: 20976–20981.
  30. 30. Barad M, Bourtchouladze R, Winder DG, Golan H, Kandel E (1998) Rolipram, a type IV-specific phosphodiesterase inhibitor, facilitates the establishment of long-lasting long-term potentiation and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95: 15020–15025.
  31. 31. Bach ME, Barad M, Son H, Zhuo M, Lu YF, et al. (1999) Age-related defects in spatial memory are correlated with defects in the late phase of hippocampal long-term potentiation in vitro and are attenuated by drugs that enhance the cAMP signaling pathway. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96: 5280–5285.
  32. 32. Yin JC, Del Vecchio M, Zhou H, Tully T (1995) CREB as a memory modulator: induced expression of a dCREB2 activator isoform enhances long-term memory in Drosophila. Cell 81: 107–115.
  33. 33. Perazzona B, Isabel G, Preat T, Davis RL (2004) The role of cAMP response element-binding protein in Drosophila long-term memory. J Neurosci 24: 8823–8828.
  34. 34. Yin JC, Wallach JS, Del Vecchio M, Wilder EL, Zhou H, et al. (1994) Induction of a dominant negative CREB transgene specifically blocks long-term memory in Drosophila. Cell 79: 49–58.
  35. 35. Blokland A, Schreiber R, Prickaerts J (2006) Improving memory: a role for phosphodiesterases. Curr Pharm Des 12: 2511–2523.
  36. 36. Rose GM, Hopper A, De Vivo M, Tehim A (2005) Phosphodiesterase inhibitors for cognitive enhancement. Curr Pharm Des 11: 3329–3334.
  37. 37. Wachtel H (1983) Potential antidepressant activity of rolipram and other selective cyclic adenosine 3′, 5′-monophosphate phosphodiesterase inhibitors. Neuropharmacology 22: 267–272.
  38. 38. Bourtchouladze R, Lidge R, Catapano R, Stanley J, Gossweiler S, et al. (2003) A mouse model of Rubinstein-Taybi syndrome: defective long-term memory is ameliorated by inhibitors of phosphodiesterase 4. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100: 10518–10522.
  39. 39. Rutten K, Lieben C, Smits L, Blokland A (2007) The PDE4 inhibitor rolipram reverses object memory impairment induced by acute tryptophan depletion in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 192: 275–282.
  40. 40. Egawa T, Mishima K, Matsumoto Y, Iwasaki K, Iwasaki K, et al. (1997) Rolipram and its optical isomers, phosphodiesterase 4 inhibitors, attenuated the scopolamine-induced impairments of learning and memory in rats. Jpn J Pharmacol 75: 275–281.
  41. 41. Zhang HT, Huang Y, Suvarna NU, Deng C, Crissman AM, et al. (2005) Effects of the novel PDE4 inhibitors MEM1018 and MEM1091 on memory in the radial-arm maze and inhibitory avoidance tests in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 179: 613–619.
  42. 42. Zhang HT, O'Donnell JM (2000) Effects of rolipram on scopolamine-induced impairment of working and reference memory in the radial-arm maze tests in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 150: 311–316.
  43. 43. Gong B, Vitolo OV, Trinchese F, Liu S, Shelanski M, et al. (2004) Persistent improvement in synaptic and cognitive functions in an Alzheimer mouse model after rolipram treatment. J Clin Invest 114: 1624–1634.
  44. 44. Nagakura A, Niimura M, Takeo S (2002) Effects of a phosphodiesterase IV inhibitor rolipram on microsphere embolism-induced defects in memory function and cerebral cyclic AMP signal transduction system in rats. Br J Pharmacol 135: 1783–1793.
  45. 45. Fleischhacker WW, Hinterhuber H, Bauer H, Pflug B, Berner P, et al. (1992) A multicenter double-blind study of three different doses of the new cAMP-phosphodiesterase inhibitor rolipram in patients with major depressive disorder. Neuropsychobiology 26: 59–64.
  46. 46. McLachlan CS, Chen ML, Lynex CN, Goh DL, Brenner S, et al. (2007) Changes in PDE4D isoforms in the hippocampus of a patient with advanced Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 64: 456–457.
  47. 47. Parkes JD, Thompson C, Brennan L, Gajraj N, Howcroft B, et al. (1984) Rolipram in Parkinson's disease. Adv Neurol 40: 563–565.
  48. 48. Yang L, Calingasan NY, Lorenzo BJ, Beal MF (2008) Attenuation of MPTP neurotoxicity by rolipram, a specific inhibitor of phosphodiesterase IV. Exp Neurol 211: 311–314.
  49. 49. Kanes SJ, Tokarczyk J, Siegel SJ, Bilker W, Abel T, et al. (2007) Rolipram: a specific phosphodiesterase 4 inhibitor with potential antipsychotic activity. Neuroscience 144: 239–246.
  50. 50. Siuciak JA, Chapin DS, McCarthy SA, Martin AN (2007) Antipsychotic profile of rolipram: efficacy in rats and reduced sensitivity in mice deficient in the phosphodiesterase-4B (PDE4B) enzyme. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 192: 415–424.
  51. 51. Sasaki H, Hashimoto K, Inada T, Fukui S, Iyo M (1995) Suppression of oro-facial movements by rolipram, a cAMP phosphodiesterase inhibitor, in rats chronically treated with haloperidol. Eur J Pharmacol 282: 71–76.
  52. 52. Sasaki H, Hashimoto K, Maeda Y, Inada T, Kitao Y, et al. (1995) Rolipram, a selective c-AMP phosphodiesterase inhibitor suppresses oro-facial dyskinetic movements in rats. Life Sci 56: L443–L447.
  53. 53. Anderson GM, Horne WC, Chatterjee D, Cohen DJ (1990) The hyperserotonemia of autism. Ann NY Acad Sci 600: 331–340.
  54. 54. Hranilovic D, Novak R, Babic M, Novokmet M, Bujas-Petkovic Z, et al. (2008) Hyperserotonemia in autism: the potential role of 5HT-related gene variants. Coll Antropol 32: Suppl 175–80.
  55. 55. Hranilovic D, Bujas-Petkovic Z, Tomicic M, Bordukalo-Niksic T, Blazevic S, et al. (2009) Hyperserotonemia in autism: activity of 5HT-associated platelet proteins. J Neural Transm 116: 493–501.
  56. 56. Foguet M, Hartikka JA, Schmuck K, Lubbert H (1993) Long-term regulation of serotonergic activity in the rat brain via activation of protein kinase A. EMBO J 12: 903–910.
  57. 57. Galter D, Unsicker K (2000) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and trkB are essential for cAMP-mediated induction of the serotonergic neuronal phenotype. J Neurosci Res 61: 295–301.
  58. 58. Deogracias R, Espliguero G, Iglesias T, Rodriguez-Pena A (2004) Expression of the neurotrophin receptor trkB is regulated by the cAMP/CREB pathway in neurons. Mol Cell Neurosci 26: 470–480.
  59. 59. Hjerrild M, Stensballe A, Jensen ON, Gammeltoft S, Rasmussen TE (2004) Protein kinase A phosphorylates serine 267 in the homeodomain of engrailed-2 leading to decreased DNA binding. FEBS Lett 568: 55–59.
  60. 60. Morgan R (2006) Engrailed: complexity and economy of a multi-functional transcription factor. FEBS Lett 580: 2531–2533.
  61. 61. Benayed R, Gharani N, Rossman I, Mancuso V, Lazar G, et al. (2005) Support for the homeobox transcription factor gene ENGRAILED 2 as an autism spectrum disorder susceptibility locus. Am J Hum Genet 77: 851–868.
  62. 62. Cheh MA, Millonig JH, Roselli LM, Ming X, Jacobsen E, et al. (2006) En2 knockout mice display neurobehavioral and neurochemical alterations relevant to autism spectrum disorder. Brain Res 1116: 166–176.
  63. 63. Gharani N, Benayed R, Mancuso V, Brzustowicz LM, Millonig JH (2004) Association of the homeobox transcription factor, ENGRAILED 2, 3, with autism spectrum disorder. Mol Psychiatry 9: 474–484.
  64. 64. Sen B, Singh AS, Sinha S, Chatterjee A, Ahmed S, et al. (2010) Family-based studies indicate association of Engrailed 2 gene with autism in an Indian population. Genes Brain Behav 9: 248–255.
  65. 65. Wang L, Jia M, Yue W, Tang F, Qu M, et al. (2008) Association of the ENGRAILED 2 (EN2) gene with autism in Chinese Han population. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 147B: 434–438.