Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Validity and measurement invariance across sex, age, and education level of the French short versions of the European Health Literacy Survey Questionnaire

  • Alexandra Rouquette,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Université Paris-Saclay, Univ. Paris-Sud, UVSQ, CESP, INSERM, Villejuif, France, AP-HP, Bicêtre Hôpitaux Universitaires Paris Sud, Public Health and Epidemiology Department, Le Kremlin-Bicêtre, France

  • Théotime Nadot,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Université Paris-Saclay, Univ. Paris-Sud, UVSQ, CESP, INSERM, Villejuif, France

  • Pierre Labitrie,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation Université Paris-Saclay, Univ. Paris-Sud, General Practice Department, Le Kremlin-Bicêtre, France

  • Stephan Van den Broucke,

    Roles Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain, Belgium

  • Julien Mancini,

    Roles Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Aix-Marseille Univ, INSERM, IRD, UMR1252, SESSTIM, “Cancers, Biomedicine & Society” group, Marseille, France, APHM, Timone Hospital, Public Health Department (BIOSTIC), Marseille, France

  • Laurent Rigal ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    laurent.rigal@free.fr

    Affiliations Université Paris-Saclay, Univ. Paris-Sud, UVSQ, CESP, INSERM, Villejuif, France, Université Paris-Saclay, Univ. Paris-Sud, General Practice Department, Le Kremlin-Bicêtre, France, Institut National d’Etudes Démographiques (INED), Paris, France

  • Virginie Ringa

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Université Paris-Saclay, Univ. Paris-Sud, UVSQ, CESP, INSERM, Villejuif, France, Institut National d’Etudes Démographiques (INED), Paris, France

Abstract

Background

Short versions of the European Health Literacy Survey (HLS-EU) questionnaire are increasingly used to measure and compare health literacy (HL) in populations worldwide. As no validated versions of these questionnaires have thus far appeared in French, this study aimed to study the psychometric properties of the French translation of the 16- and 6-item short versions (HLS-EU-Q16 and HLS-EU-Q6), including their measurement invariance across sex, age, and education level.

Methods

A consensual French version of the HLS-EU-Q16 and HLS-EU-Q6 was developed by following the current recommendations for transcultural questionnaire adaptation. It was then completed by 317 patients recruited in waiting rooms of general practitioners in the Paris area (France). Structural validity was studied with the Rasch model for the HLS-EU-Q16 and confirmatory factorial analysis (CFA) for the HLS-EU-Q6. Concurrent and convergent validity, respectively, were assessed by scores on the Functional Communicative Critical Health Literacy (FCCHL) questionnaire and the physicians’ evaluations of their patient’s HL.

Results

The 16 items of the HLS-EU-Q16 were Rasch homogenous but meaningful differential item functioning (DIF) was found across sex, age, and/or education level for eight items. The CFA model fit for the HLS-EU-Q6 was poor. The overall scores for both HLS-EU short versions correlated poorly with the FCCHL scores. Similarly, HL levels defined using either short-version score did not agree with physicians’ HL assessments.

Conclusion

The French version of the HLS-EU-Q16 has acceptable psychometric properties, despite meaningful DIF for age, sex and education level and a poor discriminative power among subjects with average to high HL level. We recommend its use to measure HL in populations with sufficient reading skills to discriminate between subjects with low to average HL. Also, sensitivity analyses should be performed to evaluate the potential measurement bias due to DIF. Our results did not demonstrate the validity of the HLS-EU-Q6.

Introduction

Health literacy (HL) is defined as "the cognitive and social skills which determine the motivation and ability of individuals to gain access to, understand and use information in ways which promote and maintain good health" [1]. Three dimensions are distinguished: functional literacy, which involves basic skills (reading, writing, etc.) to access health information; interactive literacy, which refers to more advanced cognitive skills to understand this information; and critical literacy, which involves in-depth cognitive and social skills that ultimately lead to better control of life events [2]. Low HL has been shown to be associated with poor health, limited survival, and a higher cost of care [37]. Furthermore, the World Health Organization emphasizes the central role of HL in addressing health inequalities worldwide [1]. Nevertheless, HL has been studied only sparsely in France, likely due to the lack of adequate measurement instruments validated in French [810].

Some screening tools for low HL, such as the Rapid Estimate of Adult Literacy in Medicine (REALM) or the Test of Functional Health Literacy in Adults (TOFHLA) have been translated in French, but they assess only functional literacy, through timed tests evaluating the recognition of medical terms or the understanding of medical texts [11,12]. More recently, however, French adaptations of broader tools are being developed. For instance, the Functional Communicative Critical Health Literacy (FCCHL) scale, based on Nutbeam’s definition, has recently been validated in French, but its use in epidemiological studies is limited because the implication of disease diagnosis in its wording (“If you are diagnosed…”) reduces its relevance in healthy populations [13]. A transcultural adaptation in French of the Health Literacy Questionnaire (HLQ), measuring nine dimensions related to individual traits and abilities as well as contextual and health system resources, has recently been published [14,15]. Another interesting tool is the European Health Literacy Survey Questionnaire (HLS-EU-Q), which is built on a conceptual model of HL developed by a European consortium (not including France) based on a review of 170 publications [16]. This model integrates four health information processing skills (accessing, understanding, appraising, and applying health information) applied in three health contexts (healthcare, disease prevention, and health promotion). These skills go well beyond functional literacy, which focuses mainly on understanding health information; they also consider its communicative (e.g., accessing and discussing this information) and critical dimensions (appraising and applying it) [17,18]. A Delphi method was used to generate and select 47 items covering the 12 domains (three health contexts x four skills) [17].

One of the main obstacles to the use of these questionnaires in epidemiological studies, however, is their length. The addition of more than 40 questions to measure HL is rarely possible in studies that already involve several other questionnaires. Although no short version of the HLQ currently exists, to our knowledge, short versions of the HLS-EU-Q, containing 16 (HLS-EU-Q16) and 6 (HLS-EU-Q6) items [18], have been developed. The 16 items of the HLS-EU-Q16 were selected among the 47 HLS-EU-Q items based on their psychometric properties evaluated by Rasch analysis and their simultaneous good face and content validity by ensuring representation of the 12 HLS-EU domains [19] (S1 Table). This questionnaire provides an overall score of HL that has been shown to be highly correlated (r = 0.82) with the overall score on the full 47-item version of the HLS-EU-Q. The 6 items of the HLS-EU-Q6 were selected from the HLS-EU-Q16 using confirmatory factorial analysis (CFA) to establish its factorial structure, and correlation coefficients with the scores on the longer versions to determine its convergent validity [20]. The HLS-EU-Q6 has been used in a limited number of clinical studies in Europe [21,22], while the HLS-EU-Q16 is increasingly used for population studies in Europe in numerous countries. It has been translated into Dutch [7,2325], Swedish [26,27], German [2830], Norwegian [31], Spanish and Catalan [32], Italian [33], Greek [34], Czech [35], Hebrew [36], and Arabic [37]. A French version of this questionnaire has been used in two studies in Belgium [7,38], but the published information regarding its psychometric properties is limited.

French is the fourth most widely spoken first language in the European Union, after German, Italian, and English. It is an official language of four European countries (France, Belgium, Switzerland, and Luxembourg) and of 25 independent nations outside Europe. As the short versions of the HLS-EU-Q are increasingly used to measure and compare HL in populations within Europe and worldwide, it is imperative to ascertain the validity of the French version of these questionnaires. As for any measurement device, measurement invariance is a required property to guarantee accurate group comparisons and is thus essential for questionnaire validation. According to Mokkink et al. and Milsap, “[a] measuring device should function in the same way across varied conditions, so long as those varied conditions are irrelevant to the attribute being measured” [39,40]. The objective of this study was thus to translate the HLS-EU-Q16 and HLS-EU-Q6 into French and to evaluate their psychometric properties, including measurement invariance across sex, age, and education level.

Methods

Translation

In accordance with the steps described in the current recommendations on transcultural adaptation of questionnaires [41,42], six experts from various disciplines (epidemiology, biostatistics, psychometrics, general medicine, public health, and psychiatry), including one bilingual English-French expert and five French experts with very high levels of English language proficiency, independently translated the English version of the HLS-EU-Q16 into French [43]. A consensus meeting was then held to arrive at a consensual French version of the questionnaire, based on the six independent translations and on the French version of the questionnaire previously used in Belgium (S1 Text). No back-translation was performed, as this has recently been proven unnecessary [44]. Ten subjects (4 males, mean age = 30 years) tested this version (completion time: 5 to 12 min). No formal cognitive debriefing interviews were performed but short individual discussions to assess acceptability and comprehensiveness of each item. No modification of the translated HSL-EU-Q-16 version was needed after this pilot test. The readability level of the translation, assessed using the Flesch Readability Score adapted to texts written in French, was 48, which corresponds to an undergraduate (bordering end of high-school) level [45].

Psychometric properties

Sample.

Subjects were recruited from May 15 to June 30, 2016, in the waiting rooms of 17 general practitioners involved in the general practice network of Université Paris-Sud (France). General practitioners were selected to ensure representation of the various social backgrounds that exist in the Paris area, but not statistical representativeness for either Paris or France as a whole. Explanations about the study were provided to all French-speaking patients arriving in the waiting room, aged 18 years or older. They were then asked to complete the “patient questionnaire”. At the end of the day, the physician completed a "physician questionnaire" for each patient who had participated in the study that day. All patients provided signed informed consent to participate. The institutional ethic committee (Comité d’Ethique du Collège National des Généralistes Enseignants, n°IRB IRB00010804) approved the study, that is, determined that it met the requirements of legal codes that govern health research in France.

Data collection.

Patients provided socio-demographic information including sex, age, and education level, as well as perceived health status ("Would you say that overall, your health is: excellent/very good/good/medium/poor?") and perceived financial situation ("Currently, with regard to your household financial situation, would you say that: you are very comfortable/relatively comfortable/just about managing/not really managing <often struggle to make ends meet> or not managing <often have to do without essentials or go deeper into debt>). Patients completed the French version of the HLS-EU-Q16 by indicating their response to each question on a 4-point Likert-like scale ("very easy", "easy", "difficult", "very difficult") for each item. To study the concurrent validity, we measured functional, communicative, and critical HL with the French version of the FCCHL. In addition, the physician answered one question about each patient: “In your opinion, this patient’s level of HL is: inadequate/medium/satisfactory?”. Apart from the World Health Organization’s definition of HL [1], no specific criteria were provided to practitioners to answer this question.

Statistical analyses.

Answers for each of the 16 items of the HLS-EU-Q16 were re-scored in reverse so that higher scores reflected higher levels of HL. Ceiling and floor effects were identified for each item; these two effects were defined a priori as respectively more than 95% of respondents who select the highest and the lowest category.

The structural validity of the 16- and 6-item versions of the HLS-EU-Q was evaluated by using the same statistical strategy used in the initial validation studies [18,20]. Specifically, a Rasch analysis was used for the HLS-EU-Q16, with dichotomized items (the "very easy" category was merged with the "easy" category, and the "difficult" category with the "very difficult" category). A monotonely homogeneous model of Mokken was fitted to verify the three fundamental assumptions (unidimensionality, local independence, monotonicity) on which the Rasch model relies. Its fit was considered acceptable if the Loevinger H coefficients were >0.3 for the H coefficient of scalability and for the Hj coefficients associated with each item j (j = 1,…,16) and were >0 for the Hjk coefficients associated with each pair of items j and k [46]. The global fit of the Rasch model was evaluated with a Chi2 test, and individual item fit with standardized residuals (expected to be ± 2.5) and Chi2 tests. The dimensional structure of the HLS-EU-Q6 was studied by using CFA on the reversed 4-point-Likert-like items and as the robust estimator for categorical data being the Weighted least square Means and Variances adjusted. Two models were fitted: a one-factor model and a two-order model with three factors according to health contexts and a higher order factor for global HL. Fit indices used were the comparative fit and Tucker-Lewis indices (CFI & TLI, good fit if >0.95, poor fit if <0.90, acceptable fit elsewhere) and the root mean square error approximation (RMSEA, good fit if <0.06, poor fit if >0.1, acceptable fit elsewhere) [47].

The Rasch analyses allowed us to assess measurement invariance which holds if two subjects being identical on the measured construct but from different groups (males and females, for example) have the same probability of giving any particular answer to any item of the scale [40,48]. If measurement invariance does not hold it means that one or several items of the scale “functions” differently in the groups to be compared (resulting in the Rasch model in different item parameter, termed difficulty, in the two groups) and that group comparisons of the total scale score may be inaccurate; this phenomenon is termed differential item functioning (DIF) [48,49]. Two kinds of DIF can be distinguished: uniform if the relation between the group and the response to the item is identical at every level of the latent trait (HL); otherwise DIF is non-uniform [48]. These both kinds of DIF were investigated in the HLS-EU-Q16 across sex, age (categorized based on tertiles) and educational level (primary or none, secondary, post-secondary) [50]. When statistically significant DIF was observed, the item was split in pseudo-items to estimate its difficulty in each group. DIF was considered meaningful if the difference in item difficulties across groups was higher than 0.25 logit or if more than 25% of the items of the scale were affected by DIF in the same direction. When DIF affected several items in opposite directions, the expected difference in the scale score across groups due to DIF was evaluated [51]. Internal consistency was assessed with the Cronbach alpha coefficient (acceptable if higher than 0.7) [52].

To assess concurrent validity, the overall HLS-EU-Q16 score was computed as the simple sum score of the 16 binary items, while the overall HLS-EU-Q6 score was computed by averaging the responses to the six items on the reversed four-point Likert scale, both as recommended for other language versions [20]. The three levels of HL were the same as those in the other language versions: inadequate (HLS-EU-Q16 score ≤8, HLS-EU-Q6 score ≤2), problematic (HLS-EU-Q16 score >8 and ≤12, HLS-EU-Q6 score >2 and ≤3), and adequate (HLS-EU-Q16 score >12, HLS-EU-Q6 score >3) [18,20,53]. The association between the overall HLS-EU-Q16 and HLS-EU-Q6 scores was estimated with the Spearman correlation coefficient, and the kappa coefficient was used to evaluate agreement (acceptable if kappa>0.6, excellent if >0.8) between HL levels determined by the HLS-EU-Q16 and HLS-EU-Q6 [54]. Spearman coefficients were also used to evaluate the association of both HLS-EU overall scores with the FCCHL functional, communicative and critical HL scores, and the kappa coefficient was used to evaluate the agreement between the HL levels obtained for each patient by the HLS-EU-Q16 and HLS-EU-Q6 with the level evaluated by the physician.

To determine the questionnaires’ convergent and discriminant validity, comparisons were made between patients depending on their education level, perceived health status, and financial situation, and HL as evaluated by their physician. Lower HL was expected for less educated patients, those with poorer perceived health status, poorer perceived financial situation, and low physician-assessed HL level [5558]. These a priori hypotheses were tested with Mann-Whitney tests. The kappa coefficient was also used to evaluate the agreement between the HL level determined with the HLS-EU-Q16 and HLS-EU-Q6, and the physician-assessed HL level. Analyses were performed with the Stata v.14 (data management and basic statistics), RUMM2030 (Rasch analyses), and Mplus v7.4 (CFA) software [5961].

Results

Subjects

Of the 372 patients who were approached for the study, 343 agreed to participate (response rate: 92%); 26 (8%) were subsequently excluded due to a missing answer on one or more of the HLS-EU-Q16 items. Table 1 summarizes the socio-demographic characteristics of the remaining 317 patients; 207 (65%) were women, their mean age was 53 (±18) years and 188 (59%) had a post-secondary education level. In all, 216 (68%) assessed their financial situation as “very comfortable” or “relatively comfortable” and 208 (66%) rated their health as "good", "very good" or "excellent".

Psychometric properties of the French version of the HLS-EU-Q16 and HL-SEU-Q6

Descriptive analyses and floor and ceiling effects.

None of the HLS-EU-Q16 items had floor or ceiling effects when the 4-point Likert scale was used (S2 Table). After they were dichotomized, however, ceiling effects were observed for four items: item 3 (understanding your doctor), 4 (understanding your doctor’s or pharmacist’s instructions), 7 (following your doctor’s or pharmacist’s instructions), and 10 (understanding why you need health screening tests). The distributions of the scores on the HLS-EU-Q16 and HLS-EU-Q6 are reported in Table 2. A ceiling effect was observed for the HLS-EU-Q16 score, with 80 (25%) patients scoring 16. When the scores were categorised, the HL level was defined as inadequate for 26 (8%) and 16 (5%) subjects, problematic for 106 (33%) and 218 (69%), and adequate for 185 (58%) and 83 (26%) with the HLS-EU-Q16 and HLS-EU-Q6, respectively.

thumbnail
Table 2. Distribution of the scores on European Health Literacy Survey Questionnaire with 16 items (HLS-EU-Q16) and with 6 items (HLS-EU-Q6) in the overall sample and according to physician’s evaluation of patient health literacy (HL), education level, perceived health status, and perceived financial situation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208091.t002

Rasch analyses and study of the measurement invariance of the HLS-EU-Q16.

Loevinger’s H coefficients confirmed the unidimensionality, local independence and monotonicity hypotheses, except for the H coefficient (0.28) associated with item 1 (finding information on treatments). The overall Chi2 test P-value for the Rasch model fit was 0.08. Standardized fit residuals and Chi2 tests indicated that the Rasch model had a good fit at the item level, as summarized in Table 3. Item difficulty varied from -2.42 to 2.18, and the latent trait (HL) level of more than 40% of the sample was higher than the highest item’s difficulty, as shown on the person-item map (S1 Fig).

thumbnail
Table 3. Item fit of the French short version of the European Health Literacy Survey Questionnaire with 16 items (HLS-EU-Q16), with the Rasch model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208091.t003

Table 4 presents the results from the DIF analyses. Item 1 (finding information on treatments) showed meaningful DIF across sex can be interpreted as follow: if men and women have the same HL level, men respond more often than women that it is difficult to find information on treatments of illnesses that concern them. Three items (item 3: understanding what your doctor says; item 5: judging when you may need to get a second opinion; and item 14: understand advice on health from family members or friends) showed meaningful DIF across age. At the same HL level, older people respond more often than younger that it is difficult to understand what the doctor says and to judge when they need to get a second opinion. Persons between 41 and 60 respond more often that it is easy to understand advice on health from relatives and friends compared to younger or older people. Education level was associated with a meaningful DIF: two items were easier for more educated patients (item 1: finding information on treatments, and item 6: using information the doctor gives you to make decisions), and two more difficult (item 11: judging if the information on health risks in the media is reliable, and item 12: deciding how you can protect yourself from illness based on information in the media). At the same HL level, more educated subjects answer more often that it is difficult to find information on treatments and to use information given by the doctor to make decisions. To the contrary, they answer less that it is difficult to judge the reliability of the information in the media and to decide how to protect themselves based on information in the media. As DIF was not in the same direction for these five items, a higher HLS-EU-Q16 score was expected for more educated than for less educated subjects when the latent trait (HL level) was low, while a lower score was expected when the HL level was high (S2 Fig). The expected difference of the HLS-EU-Q16 score due to DIF across education levels reached a maximum of 0.54 points between the primary and post-secondary education levels for a latent trait level of 2 logit (i.e., an expected score of 13.55 and of 13.01 in the primary and post-secondary education levels respectively).

thumbnail
Table 4. Items from the HLS-EU-Q16 with meaningful differential item functioning across sex, age, or education level.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208091.t004

CFA of the HL-SEU-Q6 questionnaire.

Indices of fit for the one-factor CFA model applied on the reversed 4-point Likert scale were: CFI = 0.948; TLI = 0.913, and RMSEA = 0.176 (90% confidence interval, 0.145; 0.208). Computational issues (non-positive information matrix) precluded a reliable assessment of fit indices for the two-order CFA model.

Internal consistency.

The Cronbach alpha coefficients were 0.81 and 0.83 for the HLS-EU-Q16 and the HLS-EU-Q6, respectively.

Concurrent validity.

The Spearman correlation between the HLS-EU-Q16 and HLS-EU-Q6 scores was 0.88. In contrast, the agreement between HL levels defined by both of these versions was poor, with a Kappa coefficient equal to 0.36. In addition, the Spearman correlation coefficients of the scores on both these versions with the functional, communicative and critical HL scales of the FCCHL were statistically significant (P-value<0.05) but all below 0.3 (range: 0.11–0.29).

Convergent and discriminant validity.

Results from the a priori hypotheses-tests are shown in Table 2. No significant differences were observed between patients according to their education level or perceived health status. A trend was observed by which the HLS-EU-Q16 score decreased with perceived health status, but not for the HLS-EU-Q6. HL measures were not associated with education level, even when the score was computed without the five items affected by DIF concerning education. On the other hand, a significant association was found between HL and perceived financial situation. Physicians evaluated the HL as insufficient for 26 (9%) patients, medium for 81 (28%) and satisfactory for 179 (63%). On average, the overall HLS-EU-Q16 and HLS-EU-Q6 scores were higher with higher physician-assessed HL (P-values = 0.002 and 0.033, respectively) (Table 4). Nonetheless, the agreement between the HL categories as evaluated by physicians and using the questionnaires was poor, with Kappa coefficients equal to 0.10 for the HLS-EU-Q16 and 0.06 for the HLS-EU-Q6.

Discussion

Transcultural adaptation and validation of the short forms of the HLS-EU questionnaire are necessary steps before they can be used to measure HL at the population level among French-speaking subjects and then to compare HL levels across populations, which was the primary aim of the HLS-EU study [17].

Our results indicate that the French version of the HLS-EU-Q16 is Rasch homogenous, which is a highly recommended property for composite measurement scales. Its internal consistency is also satisfactory. On the other hand, our analyses also revealed certain limitations that suggest the need for some caution when using this form of the questionnaire. First, four items showed ceiling effects when dichotomized, which suggests that these items are not sufficiently discriminant in this population. The Rasch analysis and the ceiling effect observed for the total score are consistent with this observation and indicate that the scale based on dichotomization of the items is not sufficiently discriminatory for use among subjects with high levels of HL. As such, the French version of the questionnaire appears more appropriate for the study of people and groups with relatively low levels of HL, to discriminate between them. This finding also raises the question whether any HLSEU-Q16 items should be treated as binary items, since when scored on a four-point Likert scale, they did not show any ceiling effects.

Second, measurement invariance did not hold as DIF was observed for sex, age, and education level. Different explanations can enlighten this phenomenon. Women are more concerned about their health than men. They have more medical encounters and are more likely to seek medical treatment. Moreover, physicians spend more time with female patients and give them more explanations [62]. This specific relationship to the health care system may explain why women declare that finding information on treatments is easier than men.

Two items were more difficult for older people (understand what the doctor says and to judge when they need to get a second opinion). This may be due, for example, to age related hearing or cognitive impairments that make understanding and judgement more complicated without modifying the HL level at all. The finding that more educated subjects answer more often that it is difficult to find information on treatments and to use information given by the doctor to make decisions may be related to the known relationship between education and information seeking or preference for decision making [63,64]. While some degree of DIF is often found in questionnaire validation studies, ignoring the presence of this phenomenon could lead to biased results [51]. The DIF is particularly noticeable for education level, where it affects five items in opposite directions. Because the score difference across education levels due to DIF amounts to 0.5 units on the HLS-EU-Q16 total score, it can lead to underestimating the educational gradient in HL. The overall good fit of the Rasch model means that it is the same measured latent trait (HL) across groups but the presence of DIF signifies that it is not measured in the same way across groups. To counteract this bias, it is recommended that studies using the questionnaire in populations that vary in terms of sex, age, or (especially) education level perform sensitivity analyses by calculating the HLS-EU-Q16 scores with and without the items showing DIF. To our knowledge, this study is the first to investigate DIF for the HLS-EU questionnaire. Evidence of the amplitude of the biases this phenomenon may produce is necessary to allow a well-informed use of the questionnaire. We therefore recommend that it be investigated in other language versions.

The results for convergent and divergent validity were consistent with our a priori hypotheses, except for that concerning education level, which was not related to the total score on the HLS-EU-Q16. This may be due to the readability level of the French version of this questionnaire, which corresponded to that of a university undergraduate. It might have induced a selection bias by discouraging respondents with lower education levels from completing the questionnaire.

Finally, correlations between the overall HLS-EU-Q16 score and the FCCHL scores were very low, as was the agreement between the HL categories determined by the HLS-EU-Q16 and by the physician. This suggests that what is measured by the HLS-EU-Q16 is different from HL as measured by the FCCHL or as conceptualized by French physicians. With regard to the latter, nonetheless, we note that many of the doctors who participated in the study were unfamiliar with the concept of HL; this lack of knowledge probably explains the low level of agreement. Correlation with the FCCHL was also low, perhaps because the latter questionnaire focuses mainly on HL in the patient-clinician interaction, and less on the health information processing skills of accessing, understanding, appraising, and applying health information in disease prevention and health promotion, which are important objects of the HLS-EU-Q. Further research exploring the links and differences between the constructs measured by the various existing health literacy measurement instruments would be helpful in choosing the most suitable questionnaire for each study.

The validity of the French version of the HLS-EU-Q6 could not be established, due to the poor fit of the one-factor CFA model and our inability to estimate the fit of a two-order CFA model reliably, due to computation issues. The Spearman correlation between the overall HLS-EU-Q16 and HLS-EU-Q6 scores was nevertheless high, suggesting that both tools measure the same construct. On the other hand, the agreement between HL levels measured by the HLS-EU-Q16 and HLS-EU-Q6 was poor, which suggests that different thresholds may be needed to categorize the overall HLS-EU-Q6 score. Moreover, the results from the analyses regarding convergent and discriminant validity were not convincing. To our knowledge, no studies have examined the psychometric properties of this short version, in any language, although it has already been used in some epidemiological studies [21,22]. Further studies should be planned to evaluate the validity of the HLS-EU-Q6 in other languages.

This study nonetheless has limitations. The sample size could be perceived as a weakness, although a sample size of 200 persons has been recommended for Rasch analysis [65]. In addition, although the entire French population has access to primary care without social differences, the method for participant recruitment via the waiting room probably resulted in over representing women and elderly people in the sample. Accordingly, inadequate specification of the psychometric properties of the French version of the HLS-EU short versions due to this selection bias cannot be ruled out. Moreover, we used a sample of participants from the Paris area, so our results must be replicated on samples of subjects from other French-speaking areas to evaluate their robustness. Finally, the use of self-administered questionnaires is limited to people who can read French well, and ad hoc studies should be planned to develop self-administered measurement tools that can be used in populations with poor reading skills.

Conclusion

Despite these limitations, we conclude that the psychometric properties of the French version of the HLS-EU-Q16 enable its use in surveys of health literacy, provided that the population surveyed has sufficient reading skills (preferably not lower than high-school level), and that it is mainly suitable to discriminate between subjects with low to average HL level. Sensitivity analyses should also be performed to evaluate the role of potential measurement bias due to DIF related to sex, age, and education level in the HLS-EU-Q16. Furthermore, as measurement invariance has rarely been studied in the field of HL assessment [13], we suggest that further studies should assess this property in every language version of the HLS-EU questionnaires, as well as for other HL measurement instruments commonly used, such as the HLQ, REALM and TOFHLA. Finally, the validity of the HLS-EU-Q6 could not be established in this study.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Items from the European Health Literacy Survey Questionnaire short forms, 16 items (HLS-EU-Q16) and 6 items (HLS-EU-Q6, in bold, and the health contexts and health-information-processing skills to which they apply.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208091.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Frequencies (%) of responses to the 16 items of the European Health Literacy Survey Questionnaire short forms, HLS-EU-Q16 and HLS-EU-Q6 (in bold), in the sample (N = 317).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208091.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table. HLSEUvalidFrench.xls is the database used in this study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208091.s003

(XLS)

S1 Fig. Rasch person-item map of the European Health Literacy Survey Questionnaire with 16 items (HLS-EU-Q16) in the sample (N = 317).

A higher (positive) location value indicates higher health literacy of the persons or greater item difficulty (logit scale).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208091.s004

(DOCX)

S2 Fig. Expected score on the European Health Literacy Survey Questionnaire with 16 items depending on education level and expected difference in score due to differential item functioning across education levels (reference “Post-secondary”) as a function of latent trait (as an example, for subjects with a health literacy level at 2 logits on the latent trait scale, their scores are expected to be 13.6, 13.3 and 13.0 on average, respectively, in the primary, secondary and post-secondary education level groups).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208091.s005

(DOCX)

S1 Text. French version of the European Health Literacy Survey Questionnaire short form with 16 items (HLS-EU-Q16).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208091.s006

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Laura Pryor, Cédric Lemogne and Gwenaël Domenech-Dorca who were involved in the translation process.

References

  1. 1. WHO WHO. Health Literacy. The Solid Facts [Internet]. 1 Jul 2013 [cited 1 Jun 2017]. http://www.thehealthwell.info/search-results/health-literacy-solid-facts?&content=resource&member=572160&catalogue=none&collection=none&tokens_complete=true
  2. 2. Nutbeam D. The evolving concept of health literacy. Soc Sci Med. 2008;67: 2072–2078. pmid:18952344
  3. 3. Cho YI, Lee S-YD, Arozullah AM, Crittenden KS. Effects of health literacy on health status and health service utilization amongst the elderly. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66: 1809–1816. pmid:18295949
  4. 4. Mantwill S, Schulz P. Low health literacy associated with higher medication costs in patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus: Evidence from matched survey and health insurance data. Patient Educ Couns. 2015; http://europepmc.org/abstract/med/26198546
  5. 5. Berkman ND, Sheridan SL, Donahue KE, Halpern DJ, Crotty K. Low health literacy and health outcomes: an updated systematic review. Ann Intern Med. 2011;155: 97–107. pmid:21768583
  6. 6. Bostock S, Steptoe A. Association between low functional health literacy and mortality in older adults: longitudinal cohort study. BMJ. 2012;344: e1602. pmid:22422872
  7. 7. Vandenbosch J, dev Broucke SV, Vancorenland S, Avalosse H, Verniest R, Callens M. Health literacy and the use of healthcare services in Belgium. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2016;. pmid:27116951
  8. 8. Balcou-Debussche M. Littératie en santé et interactions langagières en éducation thérapeutique. Analyse de situations d’apprentissage au Mali, à La Réunion et à Mayotte. Éducation Santé Sociétés. 2014;1: 3–18.
  9. 9. Balcou-Debussche M. Interroger la littératie en santé en perspective de transformations individuelles et sociales. Analyse de l’évolution de 42 personnes diabétiques sur trois ans. Rech Éducations. 2016; 73–87.
  10. 10. Margat A, Andrade VD, Gagnayre R. « Health Literacy » et éducation thérapeutique du patient: Quels rapports conceptuel et méthodologique? Educ Thérapeutique Patient—Ther Patient Educ. 2014;6: 10105.
  11. 11. Davis T, Long S, Jackson R, Mayeaux E, George R, Murphy P, et al. Rapid estimate of adult literacy in medicine: a shortened screening instrument. Fam Med. 1993;25: 391–395. pmid:8349060
  12. 12. Parker RM, Baker DW, Williams MV, Nurss JR. The test of functional health literacy in adults. J Gen Intern Med. 1995;10: 537–541. pmid:8576769
  13. 13. Ousseine Y, Rouquette A, Bouhnik A, Rigal L, Ringa V, Smith AB, et al. Validation of the French version of the Functional, Communicative and Critical Health Literacy scale (FCCHL). J Patient-Rep Outcomes. 2018;2: 3.
  14. 14. Osborne RH, Batterham RW, Elsworth GR, Hawkins M, Buchbinder R. The grounded psychometric development and initial validation of the Health Literacy Questionnaire (HLQ). BMC Public Health. 2013;13: 658. pmid:23855504
  15. 15. Debussche X, Lenclume V, Balcou-Debussche M, Alakian D, Sokolowsky C, Ballet D, et al. Characterisation of health literacy strengths and weaknesses among people at metabolic and cardiovascular risk: Validity testing of the Health Literacy Questionnaire. SAGE Open Med. 2018;6. pmid:30319778
  16. 16. Sørensen K, Van den Broucke S, Fullam J, Doyle G, Pelikan J, Slonska Z, et al. Health literacy and public health: A systematic review and integration of definitions and models. BMC Public Health. 2012;12: 80. pmid:22276600
  17. 17. Sørensen K, Van den Broucke S, Pelikan JM, Fullam J, Doyle G, Slonska Z, et al. Measuring health literacy in populations: illuminating the design and development process of the European Health Literacy Survey Questionnaire (HLS-EU-Q). BMC Public Health. 2013;13: 948. pmid:24112855
  18. 18. Pelikan J, Ganahl K. Measuring Health Literacy in General Populations: Primary Findings from the HLS-EU Consortium’s Health Literacy Assessment Effort. Health Literacy. Logan R.A. and Siegel E.R. (Eds.). IOS Press; 2018.
  19. 19. Röthlin F, Pelikan J, Ganahl K. Die Gesundheitskompetenz von 15-jährigen Jugendlichen in Österreich. Abschlussbericht der österreichischen Gesundheitskompetenz Jugendstudie im Auftrag des Hauptverbands der österreichischen Sozialversicherungsträger (HVSV). Wien, Austria: Ludwig Boltzmann Institut Health Promotion Research (LBIHPR); 2013.
  20. 20. Pelikan JM. Measuring comprehensive health literacy in general populations–The HLS-EU Instrument [Internet]. 2014 8.10; Taipeh. https://www.google.fr/url?sa=t&rct=j&q=&esrc=s&source=web&cd=2&ved=0ahUKEwi1vZr434rRAhXC7RQKHVsqCrYQFggkMAE&url=http%3A%2F%2Fwww.ahla-asia.org%2Fincludes%2Ffile_down.php%3Ffile%3D..%252Fpublic%252Fperd%252Fchinese%252FPelikan-Taipeh%2BHLS-EU-10-14%2Bfinfin.pdf&usg=AFQjCNEF65mwRACr88iBBMkretzecMQ0RQ&sig2=SjqT7K5zuOGmAfuffIW6iA&cad=rja
  21. 21. Schinckus L, Dangoisse F, Van den Broucke S, Mikolajczak M. When knowing is not enough: Emotional distress and depression reduce the positive effects of health literacy on diabetes self-management. Patient Educ Couns. 2017; pmid:28855062
  22. 22. Vandenbosch J, Van den Broucke S, Schinckus L, Schwartz P, Doyle G, Pelikan J, et al. The Impact of Health Literacy on Diabetes Self-Management Education. Health Educ J.
  23. 23. Fransen MP, Leenaars KEF, Rowlands G, Weiss BD, Maat HP, Essink-Bot M-L. International application of health literacy measures: Adaptation and validation of the newest vital sign in The Netherlands. Patient Educ Couns. 2014;97: 403–409. pmid:25224314
  24. 24. Pander Maat H, Essink-Bot M-L, Leenaars KE, Fransen MP. A short assessment of health literacy (SAHL) in the Netherlands. BMC Public Health. 2014;14. pmid:25246170
  25. 25. Storms H, Claes N, Aertgeerts B, Van den Broucke S. Measuring health literacy among low literate people: an exploratory feasibility study with the HLS-EU questionnaire. BMC Public Health. 2017;17: 475. pmid:28526009
  26. 26. Wångdahl J, Lytsy P, Mårtensson L, Westerling R. Health literacy and refugees’ experiences of the health examination for asylum seekers–a Swedish cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2015;15: 1162. pmid:26596793
  27. 27. Wångdahl J, Lytsy P, Mårtensson L, Westerling R. Health literacy among refugees in Sweden–a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2014;14: 1030. pmid:25278109
  28. 28. Tiller D, Herzog B, Kluttig A, Haerting J. Health literacy in an urban elderly East-German population–results from the population-based CARLA study. BMC Public Health. 2015;15: 883. pmid:26357978
  29. 29. Gerich J, Moosbrugger R. Subjective Estimation of Health Literacy-What Is Measured by the HLS-EU Scale and How Is It Linked to Empowerment? Health Commun. 2016; 1–10. pmid:28033479
  30. 30. Halbach SM, Enders A, Kowalski C, Pförtner T-K, Pfaff H, Wesselmann S, et al. Health literacy and fear of cancer progression in elderly women newly diagnosed with breast cancer—A longitudinal analysis. Patient Educ Couns. 2016;99: 855–862. pmid:26742608
  31. 31. Gele AA, Pettersen KS, Torheim LE, Kumar B. Health literacy: the missing link in improving the health of Somali immigrant women in Oslo. BMC Public Health. 2016;16: 1134. pmid:27809815
  32. 32. Contel JC, Ledesma A, Blay C, Mestre AG, Cabezas C, Puigdollers M, et al. Chronic and integrated care in Catalonia. Int J Integr Care. 2015;15. Available: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4491324/
  33. 33. Lorini C, Santomauro F, Grazzini M, Mantwill S, Vettori V, Lastrucci V, et al. Health literacy in Italy: a cross-sectional study protocol to assess the health literacy level in a population-based sample, and to validate health literacy measures in the Italian language. BMJ Open. 2017;7: e017812. pmid:29138204
  34. 34. Efthymiou A, Middleton N, Charalambous A, Papastavrou E. The Association of Health Literacy and Electronic Health Literacy With Self-Efficacy, Coping, and Caregiving Perceptions Among Carers of People With Dementia: Research Protocol for a Descriptive Correlational Study. JMIR Res Protoc. 2017;6: e221. pmid:29133284
  35. 35. HAJDUCHOVÁ H, BÁRTLOVÁ S, BRABCOVÁ I, MOTLOVÁ L, ŠEDOVÁ L, TÓTHOVÁ V. Zdravotní gramotnost seniorů a její vliv na zdraví a ćerpání zdravotních slušeb. Prakt Lék. 2017.
  36. 36. Levin-Zamir D, Baron-Epel OB, Cohen V, Elhayany A. The Association of Health Literacy with Health Behavior, Socioeconomic Indicators, and Self-Assessed Health From a National Adult Survey in Israel. J Health Commun. 2016;21: 61–68. pmid:27669363
  37. 37. Almaleh R, Helmy Y, Farhat E, Hasan H, Abdelhafez A. Assessment of health literacy among outpatient clinics attendees at Ain Shams University Hospitals, Egypt: a cross-sectional study. Public Health. 2017;151: 137–145. pmid:28800559
  38. 38. Van den Broucke S, Renwart. La littéracie en santé en Belgique: un médiateur des inégalités sociales et des comportements de santé. Louvain-la-Neuve: Université catholique de Louvain Faculté de psychologie et des sciences de l’éducation Institut de recherche en sciences psychologiques; 2014.
  39. 39. Mokkink LB, Terwee CB, Patrick DL, Alonso J, Stratford PW, Knol DL, et al. The COSMIN study reached international consensus on taxonomy, terminology, and definitions of measurement properties for health-related patient-reported outcomes. J Clin Epidemiol. 2010;63: 737–745. pmid:20494804
  40. 40. Millsap RE. Statistical Approaches to Measurement Invariance. 1 edition. New York: Routledge; 2011.
  41. 41. Wild D, Grove A, Martin M, Eremenco S, McElroy S, Verjee-Lorenz A, et al. Principles of Good Practice for the Translation and Cultural Adaptation Process for Patient-Reported Outcomes (PRO) Measures: Report of the ISPOR Task Force for Translation and Cultural Adaptation. Value Health. 2005;8: 94–104. pmid:15804318
  42. 42. de Vet HCW, Terwee CB, Mokkink LB, Knol DL. Measurement in Medicine: A Practical Guide [Internet]. Cambridge University Press; 2011. http://books.google.fr/books?id=_OcdeTg9i28C
  43. 43. Epstein J, Santo RM, Guillemin F. A review of guidelines for cross-cultural adaptation of questionnaires could not bring out a consensus. J Clin Epidemiol. 2015;68: 435–441. pmid:25698408
  44. 44. Epstein J, Osborne RH, Elsworth GR, Beaton DE, Guillemin F. Cross-cultural adaptation of the Health Education Impact Questionnaire: experimental study showed expert committee, not back-translation, added value. J Clin Epidemiol. 2015;68: 360–369. pmid:24084448
  45. 45. Ménoni V, Lucas N, Leforestier JF, Dimet J, Doz F, Chatellier G, et al. The Readability of Information and Consent Forms in Clinical Research in France. PLOS ONE. 2010;5: e10576. pmid:20485505
  46. 46. Sijtsma K, Molenaar IW. Introduction to Nonparametric Item Response Theory. 1 edition. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: SAGE Publications, Inc; 2002.
  47. 47. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 1999;6: 1–55.
  48. 48. Mellenbergh GJ. Item bias and item response theory. Int J Educ Res. 1989;13: 127–143.
  49. 49. Millsap RE, Everson HT. Methodology Review: Statistical Approaches for Assessing Measurement Bias. Appl Psychol Meas. 1993;17: 297–334.
  50. 50. Teresi JA, Fleishman JA. Differential item functioning and health assessment. Qual Life Res Int J Qual Life Asp Treat Care Rehabil. 2007;16 Suppl 1: 33–42. pmid:17443420
  51. 51. Rouquette A, Hardouin J-B, Coste J. Differential Item Functioning (DIF) and Subsequent Bias in Group Comparisons using a Composite Measurement Scale: a Simulation Study. J Appl Meas. 2016;17: 312–334. pmid:28027055
  52. 52. Cronbach LJ, Meehl PE. Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychol Bull. 1955;52: 281–302. pmid:13245896
  53. 53. Pelikan J, Ganahl K, Van den Broucke S, Sørensen K. Measuring Health Literacy in Europe: Introducing the European Health Literacy Survey Questionnaire (HLS-EU-Q). International Handbook of Health Literacy: Research, practice and policy across the lifespan. Bauer U., Okan O., Pinheiro P., Levin-Zamir D., Sørensen K. Bristol: Policy Press; 2018.
  54. 54. Cohen J. A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educ Psychol Meas. 1960;20: 27–46.
  55. 55. Paasche-Orlow MK, Parker RM, Gazmararian JA, Nielsen-Bohlman LT, Rudd RR. The Prevalence of Limited Health Literacy. J Gen Intern Med. 20: 175–184. pmid:15836552
  56. 56. Baker DW, Hays RD, Brook RH. Understanding changes in health status. Is the floor phenomenon merely the last step of the staircase? Med Care. 1997;35: 1–15. pmid:8998199
  57. 57. Rikard RV, Thompson MS, McKinney J, Beauchamp A. Examining health literacy disparities in the United States: a third look at the National Assessment of Adult Literacy (NAAL). BMC Public Health. 2016;16: 975. pmid:27624540
  58. 58. Kelly PA, Haidet P. Physician overestimation of patient literacy: A potential source of health care disparities. Patient Educ Couns. 2007;66: 119–122. pmid:17140758
  59. 59. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. (1998–2012). MPlus. Statistical Analysis With Latent Variables. User’s Guide. Seventh Edition [Internet]. 2012.
  60. 60. StataCorp LP. Stata Statistical Software: Release 12.1. College Station, TX: StataCorp, L.P.; 2012.
  61. 61. Andrich D, Sheridan BS, Luo G. Rumm2030: Rasch Unidimensional Measurement Models [computer software]. Perth, Western Australia: RUMM Laboratory; 2010.
  62. 62. Bird CE, Conrad P, Fremont AM. Handbook of medical sociology. Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Prentice Hall; 2000.
  63. 63. Ende J, Kazis L, Ash A, Moskowitz MA. Measuring patients’ desire for autonomy. J Gen Intern Med. 1989;4: 23–30.
  64. 64. Spies CD, Schulz CM, Weiß-Gerlach E, Neuner B, Neumann T, Dossow VV, et al. Preferences for shared decision making in chronic pain patients compared with patients during a premedication visit. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. 50: 1019–1026. pmid:16923100
  65. 65. Suen HK. Principles of Test Theories. Routledge; 2012.