Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Acute phenobarbital administration induces hyperalgesia: pharmacological evidence for the involvement of supraspinal GABA-A receptors

Abstract

The aim of the present study was to determine if phenobarbital affects the nociception threshold. Systemic (1-20 mg/kg) phenobarbital administration dose dependently induced hyperalgesia in the tail-flick, hot-plate and formalin tests in rats and in the abdominal constriction test in mice. Formalin and abdominal constriction tests were the most sensitive procedures for the detection of hyperalgesia in response to phenobarbital compared with the tail-flick and hot-plate tests. The hyperalgesia induced by systemic phenobarbital was blocked by previous administration of 1 mg/kg ip picrotoxin or either 1-2 mg/kg sc or 10 ng icv bicuculline. Intracerebroventricular phenobarbital administration (5 µg) induced hyperalgesia in the tail-flick test. In contrast, intrathecal phenobarbital administration (5 µg) induced antinociception and blocked systemic-induced hyperalgesia in this test. We suggest that phenobarbital may mediate hyperalgesia through GABA-A receptors at supraspinal levels and antinociception through the same kind of receptors at spinal levels.

phenobarbital; hyperalgesia; GABA-A receptors


Braz J Med Biol Res, March 2001, Volume 34(3) 397-405

Acute phenobarbital administration induces hyperalgesia: pharmacological evidence for the involvement of supraspinal GABA-A receptors

C.M. Yokoro, S.M.S. Pesquero, R.M.M. Turchetti-Maia, J.N. Francischi and M.A.K.F. Tatsuo

Departamento de Farmacologia, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, MG, Brasil

References

Correspondence and Footnotes Correspondence and Footnotes Correspondence and Footnotes

Abstract

The aim of the present study was to determine if phenobarbital affects the nociception threshold. Systemic (1-20 mg/kg) phenobarbital administration dose dependently induced hyperalgesia in the tail-flick, hot-plate and formalin tests in rats and in the abdominal constriction test in mice. Formalin and abdominal constriction tests were the most sensitive procedures for the detection of hyperalgesia in response to phenobarbital compared with the tail-flick and hot-plate tests. The hyperalgesia induced by systemic phenobarbital was blocked by previous administration of 1 mg/kg ip picrotoxin or either 1-2 mg/kg sc or 10 ng icv bicuculline. Intracerebroventricular phenobarbital administration (5 µg) induced hyperalgesia in the tail-flick test. In contrast, intrathecal phenobarbital administration (5 µg) induced antinociception and blocked systemic-induced hyperalgesia in this test. We suggest that phenobarbital may mediate hyperalgesia through GABA-A receptors at supraspinal levels and antinociception through the same kind of receptors at spinal levels.

Key words: phenobarbital, hyperalgesia, GABA-A receptors

Introduction

Barbiturates are drugs with the potential to reduce anxiety and promote sleep, to induce general anesthesia, and in special cases to inhibit tonic-clonic seizures (1). In some of these conditions, they have been replaced with other drugs such as benzodiazepines and serotonin re-uptake inhibitors. However, one such drug - phenobarbital - remains the drug of choice for long-term treatment of generalized (tonic-clonic) seizures in view of its effectiveness, low cost and low toxicity (2).

With respect to the mechanism of action, barbiturates are known to enhance the inhibitory effects of the neurotransmitter gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) at the GABA-A receptor level (3). To achieve this, barbiturates bind at the GABA-A receptor increasing the opening time of chloride channels, thus permitting chloride ion entry into the cells (4). In addition, activation of the GABA-A receptor has been associated with pain modulation in the central nervous system (CNS), essentially through the descending inhibitory system (5). However, it is still a matter of discussion in the literature whether barbiturates increase (6,7) or decrease (8,9) the pain threshold.

Since the barbiturate phenobarbital continues to be the drug of choice to treat tonic-clonic seizures, our aim was to investigate if phenobarbital would interfere with the nociceptive threshold. For this purpose, phenobarbital was acutely tested in four algesimetric assays using bicuculline and picrotoxin as pharmacological tools through various routes of administration.

Material and Methods

Animals

Experiments were carried out on male Holtzman rats (180-250 g) and Swiss mice (20-35 g), supplied by the Animal House of the Federal University of Minas Gerais. Animals were housed under controlled temperature (23 ± 2oC), on a 12-h dark/light cycle, with food and water ad libitum. The ethical guidelines of the International Association for the Study of Pain for investigations of experimental pain in conscious animals were followed (10).

Measurement of pain threshold

Phenobarbital alone or in combination with the inhibitors picrotoxin or bicuculline was tested in the following methods: tail flick (11), hot plate (12), and formalin (13) in rats, and abdominal constriction (14) in mice. To determine the nociception indices in the tail-flick and hot-plate tests the following formula was used: Tl - Bl/cut-off time - Bl, where Tl and Bl are test latency and baseline latency, respectively. The animals selected for testing were previously submitted to 3 sessions at 10-min intervals with baseline latencies of 3.5-4.5 s (tail flick) and 6-18 s at a temperature of 50oC (hot plate). The cut-off time for the tail-flick and hot-plate tests was 7 and 30 s, respectively. Formalin (1.25%, 50 µl) was injected into one of the hindpaws at time zero. The degree or severity of nociception is described in terms of the animal's behavior and of how it used the injected paw, as follows: degree 0: the paw touches the box, the box wall and floor; degree I: the paw touches the wall and floor lightly and the animal limps; degree II: the paw is not used and does not make contact with any surface; degree III: the animal licks, shakes or bites the paw. The nociception rate (NR) was obtained from the formula: NR = (It + IIt + IIIt/300), where t corresponds to the time (seconds) spent in each degree (I, II or III) during a period of 5 min (or 300 s) for each animal. A full abdominal constriction response to acetic acid (0.6%, v/v) was considered to be present when a wave of contraction followed by extension of the trunk and one hind limb occurred. The number of stretches was recorded during periods of 5 min over 30-min intervals for each animal in the group. The results are reported as mean number of constrictions (± SEM) for each group.

Motor coordination

Phenobarbital was also tested for motor impairment in mice conditioned to a Rotarod apparatus (Ugo Basile). The animals accepted for the test were those who fell from the Rotarod during the first 30 min of observation. The permanence time at 32 rpm was recorded for each animal before (baseline) and 15 min after intraperitoneal (ip) drug or vehicle (control) administration. The measures were made in triplicate at 5-min intervals to permit the animals to rest. The mean percentage of reduction (± SEM) in Rotarod permanence time is presented in the Results section.

Intracerebroventricular ( icv ) catheters

The rat was placed in a stereotaxic frame following pentobarbital anesthesia. A hole was trephined at coordinates overlying the left lateral ventricle, i.e., 1.4 mm posterior to the bregma and 1.5 mm left to the midline, according to the atlas of Paxinos and Watson (15). The guide cannula (an 11-mm long BD-7 stainless steel cannula) was inserted 3-3.3 mm into the lateral ventricle and fixed with a polymerized acrylic helmet adapted to the skull.

Intrathecal ( it ) catheters

Rats undergoing implantation of an it catheter were placed in a stereotaxic frame with the head flexed forward. Under pentobarbital anesthesia, a 7-cm long PE-10 tube was inserted into the subarachnoid space through a slit made in the atlanto-occipital membrane and advanced to the level of the lumbar spinal cord. The external part of the catheter was tunnelled into the skull to exit on the parietal bone. The catheter was fixed with a small piece of acrylic placed between the atlanto-occipital membrane and the skull (16).

Drug treatment

Phenobarbital was diluted in vehicle consisting of propylene glycol:saline (50:50, v/v) and administered ip 20 min before the beginning of the test or either it or icv 20 min after the beginning of the test. Picrotoxin and bicuculline were dissolved in physiological saline and administered systemically by the ip or sc route 30 min before the test. Only bicuculline was administered centrally by the icv or it route 20 min after the beginning of the test. When the drug was administered systemically (ip) the volume used was 0.1 ml/100 g for rats and 0.1 ml/10 g for mice. For the icv and it injections the volume used was 10 µl 7-10 days after cannula implantation. Icv and it injections of drugs were carefully performed over a period of 90 s to avoid intracranial hypertension or drug extravasation.

Drugs and vehicles

The following drugs were purchased from the stated sources: picrotoxin and bicuculline (Sigma Chemical Co., St. Louis, MO, USA), phenobarbital (Rhodia, Santo Amaro, SP, Brazil), propylene glycol (Reagen, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil), acetic acid (Merck, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil) and formaldehyde (38%; Labsynth, Diadema, SP, Brazil). Formaldehyde was mixed with saline to obtain a final 1.25% formalin concentration.

Statistical analysis

The results are reported as mean ± SEM. Data for the treatment groups were compared by Kruskal-Wallis analysis of variance (ANOVA) on ranks followed by the Dunnett test for multiple comparisons of nonparametric data. The level of significance was set at P<0.05 and the analyses were performed using the Sigma Stat software (version 1.199).

Results

Experiment I

Different doses of phenobarbital (2.5, 5, 10 and 20 mg/kg) or vehicle were injected ip into rats to evaluate the effects of the drug on the latency for the tail-flick reflex and the hot-plate test, as well as the nociception rate in the formalin test. In addition, the abdominal stretches in response to acetic acid were counted in mice pretreated with different doses of phenobarbital.

Systemic administration of different doses of phenobarbital (5-20 mg/kg) induced a dose-dependent reduction of the latency response in rats compared with control animals (vehicle) in the tail-flick and the hot-plate test, indicating development of hyperalgesia (Figure 1A and B; P<0.05, Kruskal-Wallis test). In addition, phenobarbital (1.25-5 mg/kg) dose dependently increased the pain score of the formalin test in rats (Figure 1C) as well as the stretch number in mice, also indicating the development of hyperalgesia (Figure 1D). A time between 40 and 60 min was necessary to observe the maximal hyperalgesic effect in the tail-flick and hot-plate tests. The maximal hyperalgesic effect occurred at 10 and 25 min after phenobarbital administration in the constriction test and formalin test, respectively. Specifically, phenobarbital increased the nociception rate both in phases 1 and 2 of the formalin-induced response (Figure 1C).

However, the minimal dose of phenobarbital needed to induce hyperalgesia varied according to the test used: 1.25 mg/kg and 2.5 mg/kg were enough to induce a significantly increased effect in the formalin and constriction tests, whereas a 2- to 8-fold increase in the dose was necessary to induce hyperalgesia in the hot-plate and the tail-flick test, respectively. In addition, reduction in the time of permanence in the Rotarod apparatus was observed in mice with increasing doses of phenobarbital (1-35 mg/kg), indicating a dose-dependent loss of motor coordination following hyperalgesia (Table 1).


Experiment II

A previous (15 min) dose of 1 mg/kg picrotoxin blocked the hyperalgesia induced by phenobarbital in all algesimetric assays performed (P<0.05, Kruskal-Wallis test). This dose of picrotoxin induced a significant antinociceptive effect when administered alone, which could be observed in the four tests studied (Figure 2A-D). Lower doses of picrotoxin (0.12 to 0.5 mg/kg) also blocked phenobarbital-induced hyperalgesia, but did not induce an antinociceptive effect per se (data not shown). Surprisingly, however, phenobarbital increased the antinociceptive effect of picrotoxin in two algesimetric tests performed in combination, i.e., the tail-flick (Figure 2A) and hot-plate (Figure 2B) tests.

An antinociceptive dose of systemic bicuculline (1-2 mg/kg, ip) blocked the hyperalgesia induced by phenobarbital both in the tail-flick and constriction tests (Figure 3A and B, respectively). In addition, phenobarbital in combination with bicuculline also potentiated the antinociceptive effect induced by the latter in the tail-flick test (Figure 3A).



Experiment III

Phenobarbital was used at a dose of 5 µg by the icv or it route to study the participation of the CNS in its hyperalgesic effect, using the tail-flick method. Intracerebroventricular administration of phenobarbital (5 µg) to rats induced development of hyperalgesia as detected by the tail-flick test (Figure 4). In contrast, the same dose of phenobarbital administered it induced an antinociceptive effect, as shown in the same figure. In addition, it administration of phenobarbital reduced the hyperalgesia induced by its systemic administration (Figure 4).

Intracerebroventricular administration of bicuculline (10 ng) blocked both the hyperalgesic effect induced by systemic phenobarbital treatment (Figure 5) and the antinociceptive effect induced by it phenobarbital administration (data not shown).



Discussion

We have previously shown that various anxiolytic drugs interfering with the GABA-A receptor, including barbiturates, could induce a state of hyperalgesia (7), although the literature about this subject may be considered controversial (8,9). Extending data from previous work (7), in the present study we showed that acute phenobarbital administration induced a dose-dependent hyperalgesia. This hyperalgesia was detected in four algesimetric assays, i.e., the tail-flick, hot-plate and formalin tests in rats, and in the abdominal constriction test in mice, thus indicating a clear-cut effect. Such effect was still more evident if one considers that at all phenobarbital doses tested a reduction of motor coordination occurred in the animals, a fact that may have theoretically impaired the observations leading to erroneous conclusions, such as a pseudo-'antinociceptive' effect, especially in mice. In fact, it should be pointed out that if higher doses of phenobarbital (>5 mg/kg) were used in the formalin or constriction test, hyperalgesia would be probably reduced by the sedation presented by the animals.

Some barbiturates are still used today therapeutically as general anesthetics (thiopental) and anticonvulsants (phenobarbital). These drugs essentially act at the CNS level, facilitating GABAergic neurotransmission by binding at specific sites in the GABA-A receptor (4). Besides controlling seizures, GABA-A receptor activation may also be involved in pain modulation (17-19). The principal nuclei of pain neuromodulation in the CNS are the periaqueductal gray matter, nucleus raphe magnus and the spinal dorsal horn which constitute the descending inhibitory system. It has been shown that stimulation of GABAergic (inhibitory) neurones associated with the periaqueductal gray matter and nucleus raphe magnus may increase the painful afferent inputs coming from the periphery (19,20), whereas activation of GABAergic neurones at the spinal dorsal horn level may have the opposite effect, i.e., a decrease of the painful peripheral inputs (21).

The potency of phenobarbital in inducing hyperalgesia varied between tests. It has been described long ago that the tail-flick response is thought to be a spinal reflex, while the hot-plate involves at least the brainstem level since coordination of the head and limbs is necessary for the response to be observed (22). The same authors also stated that the more complex behavioral pattern of the formalin test might involve other brain regions in addition to those involved in a rapid flick of the tail. Electrophysiological studies have shown that analgesia produced by stimulation of the periaqueductal gray matter requires a significantly lower current intensity in the formalin test than in the tail-flick test (23). From a pharmacological point of view, these data may suggest that a hyperalgesic dose of phenobarbital is smaller in an algesimetric assay of higher complexity. In addition, our results also support the notion that the hyperalgesic effect induced by phenobarbital is not restricted to one species, since the response was consistently detected in rats and mice.

To test if GABA-A receptors could be involved in the hyperalgesic response induced by phenobarbital, we initially chose picrotoxin - a convulsive substance which blocks the chloride channel associated specifically with the GABA-A receptor (4). Indeed, subconvulsant doses of picrotoxin inhibited the phenobarbital-induced hyperalgesic response in all tests used. This antinociceptive effect of picrotoxin has been previously observed by Tatsuo et al. (7) and may derive from an action through the GABA-A receptors present in the descending inhibitory system.

The most striking result, however, was the potentiation of the picrotoxin-induced antinociceptive response when the drug was combined with phenobarbital treatment observed in 2 out of 4 tests used, i.e., the tail-flick and hot-plate test. In fact, when 1 mg/kg picrotoxin was used in the formalin test this effect was not so clear-cut (Figure 3C), but when a lower dose was used (0.12 mg/kg) this effect could be clearly demonstrated (Yokoro CM and Tatsuo MAKF, unpublished observations). Some studies have shown that the site of barbiturate binding is close to the site of the neurotransmitter binding in the GABA-A receptor (4,5). On the other hand, the picrotoxin site is inside the channel where it seems to inhibit the entry of the chloride ions into the cell (24). Since phenobarbital potentiated the antinociceptive action of picrotoxin, it is our hypothesis that the binding of phenobarbital to its active site would allosterically change the GABA-A receptor, increasing the chance of picrotoxin binding at its site inside the channel.

Finally, the hyperalgesic effect induced by phenobarbital seems to derive from an action on upper rather than spinal levels, since hyperalgesia was observed when phenobarbital was injected by the icv but not by the it route. The fact that spinally administered phenobarbital induced an opposite (antinociceptive) effect as demonstrated in the present study, even blocking the hyperalgesia induced by (systemic) phenobarbital itself, supports this concept. Differences in subunit composition of the GABA-A receptor in supraspinal and spinal neurones (25) could account for the differences observed in the action of phenobarbital at the molecular level. This hypothesis was also supported by the demonstration that the specific GABA-A receptor antagonist bicuculline induced antinociception by the icv route in the tail-flick test (present study). Further studies from our laboratory have shown that it bicuculline administration potentiated the hyperalgesia induced by systemic administration of phenobarbital itself (Yokoro CM and Tatsuo MAKF, unpublished observations).

In conclusion, the anticonvulsant drug phenobarbital when acutely administered induced hyperalgesia within a dose range of 1.25 to 20 mg/kg in four experimental pain assays. This effect was completely blocked by subconvulsant doses of picrotoxin and bicuculline, the antagonists acting on the chloride channels associated with the GABA-A receptor and the specific receptor for the inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA, respectively, clearly implicating GABA-A receptors in this hyperalgesic effect of phenobarbital. The site for this hyperalgesic response seems to be linked with upper rather than spinal levels in the CNS, since 1) phenobarbital induces hyperalgesia when administered by the icv route, reproducing the hyperalgesic effect induced by its systemic administration; 2) it administration of the drug induces an opposite effect, i.e., an antinociceptive effect; 3) the competitive GABA-A antagonist bicuculline alone induced the opposite effect of the agonist phenobarbital, i.e., antinociception, when administered systemically (ip) or icv, and 4) bicuculline in combination with phenobarbital blocked the hyperalgesic effect induced by the latter. However, phenobarbital administered by the it route induced antinociception. Since GABA-A receptors are associated with modulation of the descending inhibitory system at upper levels of the CNS, we suggest that the phenobarbital-induced hyperalgesic response may derive from an inhibitory effect on this system, favoring the painful inputs coming from the periphery. We also suggest that phenobarbital may induce an antinociceptive effect through an effect at the spinal level also involving GABA-A receptors.

Address for correspondence: M.A.K.F. Tatsuo, Departamento de Farmacologia, ICB, UFMG, Av. Antônio Carlos, 6627, 31270-100 Belo Horizonte, MG, Brasil. Fax: +55-31-499-2695. E-mail: funayama@mono.icb.ufmg.br

Research supported by FAPEMIG and CNPq. Received January 4, 2000. Accepted December 12, 2000.

  • 1. Hobbs WR, Rall TR & Verdoorn TA (1996). Hypnotics and sedative; ethanol. In: Hardman JG, Gilman AG & Limbird LE (Editors), The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics 9th edn. MacGraw-Hill Companies, Inc., New York.
  • 2. MacNamara JO (1996). Drugs effective in the therapy of epilepsies. In: Hardman JG, Gilman AG & Limbird LE (Editors), The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics 9th edn. MacGraw-Hill Companies, Inc., New York.
  • 3. Matsumoto RR (1989). GABA receptors: are cellular differences reflected in function? Brain Research Reviews, 14: 203-225.
  • 4. MacDonald RL & Olsen RW (1994). GABA-A receptor channels. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 17: 569-602.
  • 5. Drower EJ & Hammond DL (1988). GABAergic modulation of nociceptive threshold: Effects of THIP and bicuculline microinjected in the ventral medulla of the rat. Brain Research, 450: 316-324.
  • 6. Franklin KBJ & Abbott FV (1993). Pentobarbital, diazepam, and ethanol abolish the interphase diminution of pain in the formalin test: Evidence for pain modulation by GABA-A receptors. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 46: 661-666.
  • 7. Tatsuo MAKF, Yokoro CM, Salgado JV, Pesquero SMS, Santana MAP & Francischi JN (1997). Hyperalgesic effect induced by barbiturates, midazolam and ethanol: pharmacological evidence for GABA-A receptor involvement. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 30: 251-256.
  • 8. González-Darder JM, Ortega-Alvaro A, Ruz-Franzi L & Segura-Pastor D (1992). Antinociceptive effects of phenobarbital in "tail flick" test and deafferentation pain. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 75: 81-86.
  • 9. Stein C, Morgan MM & Liebeskind JC (1987). Barbiturate-induced inhibition of a spinal nociceptive reflex: role of GABA mechanisms and descending modulation. Brain Research, 407: 307-311.
  • 10. Zimmerman M (1983). Ethical guidelines for investigations of experimental pain in conscious animals. Pain, 16: 109-110.
  • 11. D'Amour FE & Smith DL (1941). A method for determining loss of pain sensation. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 72: 74-79.
  • 12. Woolfe G & MacDonald AD (1944). The evaluation of the analgesic action of pethidine hydrochloride (demerol). Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 80: 300-307.
  • 13. Dubuisson D & Dennis SG (1977). The formalin test: a quantitative study of the analgesic effects of morphine, meperidine, and brain stem stimulation in rats and cats. Pain, 4: 161-174.
  • 14. Koster R, Anderson M & Beer EJ (1959). Acetic acid for analgesic screening. Federation Proceedings, 18: 412.
  • 15. Paxinos G & Watson C (1986). The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates 2nd edn. Academic Press, Inc., Sydney.
  • 16. Yaksh TL & Rudy TA (1976). Chronic catheterization of the spinal subarachnoid space. Physiology and Behavior, 17: 1031-1036.
  • 17. Nicoll RA, Alger BE & Jahr CE (1980). Enkephalin blocks inhibitory pathways in the vertebrate CNS. Nature, 287: 22-25.
  • 18. Moreau JL & Fields HL (1986). Evidence for GABA involvement in midbrain control of medullary neurons that modulate nociceptive transmission. Brain Research, 397: 37-46.
  • 19. Ding XH, Ji XQ & Tsou K (1990). Pentobarbital selectively blocks supraspinal morphine analgesia. Evidence for GABA-A receptor involvement. Pain, 43: 371-376.
  • 20. Lin Q, Peng Y & Willis WD (1994). Glycine and GABA-A antagonists reduce the inhibition of primate spinothalamic tract neurons produced by stimulation in periaqueductal gray. Brain Research, 654: 286-292.
  • 21. Basbaum AI (1995). Functional analysis of the cytochemistry of the spinal dorsal horn. In: Fields HL, Dubner R & Gervero F (Editors), Advances in Pain Research and Therapy Raven Press, New York.
  • 22. Abbott FV, Melzack R & Samuel C (1982). Morphine analgesia in the tail flick and formalin pain tests is mediated by different neural systems. Experimental Neurology, 75: 644-651.
  • 23. Dennis SG, Choiniere M & Melzak R (1980). Stimulation-produced analgesia in rats: assessment with two pain tests and correlation with self-stimulation. Experimental Neurology, 68: 295-309.
  • 24. Trifiletti RR, Snowman AM & Snyder SH (1984). Barbiturate recognition site on the GABA/benzodiazepine complex is distinct from the picrotoxinin/TBPS recognition site. European Journal of Pharmacology, 106: 441-447.
  • 25. McKernan RM & Whiting PJ (1996). Which GABAA-receptor subtypes really occur in the brain? Trends in Neurosciences, 19: 139-143.
  • Correspondence and Footnotes

  • Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      16 Mar 2001
    • Date of issue
      Mar 2001

    History

    • Accepted
      12 Dec 2000
    • Received
      04 Jan 2000
    Associação Brasileira de Divulgação Científica Av. Bandeirantes, 3900, 14049-900 Ribeirão Preto SP Brazil, Tel. / Fax: +55 16 3315-9120 - Ribeirão Preto - SP - Brazil
    E-mail: bjournal@terra.com.br