Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 08 January 2014
Sec. Schizophrenia
This article is part of the Research Topic In search of Mechanisms: Gene X Environmental Interactions in Severe Mental Disorders View all 7 articles

A Systematic Review of Cognitive Function in First-Episode Psychosis, Including a Discussion on Childhood Trauma, Stress, and Inflammation

  • 1Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
  • 2NORMENT, K.G. Jebsen Psychosis Research Unit, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
  • 3Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, UK
  • 4NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health, South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, UK
  • 5Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, UK

Objective: To carry out a systematic review of the literature addressing cognitive functions in first-episode psychosis (FEP), divided into domains. Although this is not a full “cognitive-genetics-in-schizophrenia review,” we will also include putative ideas of mechanism(s) behind these impairments, focusing on how early stress, and genetic vulnerability may moderate cognitive function in psychosis.

Method: Relevant studies were identified via computer literature searches for research published up to and including January 2013, only case-control studies were included for the neurocognitive meta-analysis.

Results: Patients with FEP present global cognitive impairment compared to healthy controls. The largest effect size was observed for verbal memory (Cohen’s d effect size = 2.10), followed by executive function (effect size = 1.86), and general IQ (effect size = 1.71). However, effect sizes varied between studies.

Conclusion: Cognitive impairment across domains, up to severe level based on Cohen’s effect size, is present already in FEP studies. However, differences in levels of impairment are observed between studies, as well as within domains, indicating that further consolidation of cognitive impairment over the course of illness may be present. Cognitive abnormalities may be linked to a neurodevelopmental model including increased sensitivity to the negative effect of stress, as well as genetic vulnerability. More research on this field is needed.

Introduction

With an increased awareness of the extent of cognitive impairments present in schizophrenia, cognitive dysfunction is now viewed by many as a core abnormality of the disorder (1, 2). The majority of patients with schizophrenia function at a cognitive level of at least one standard deviation below that of healthy comparison groups (3, 4). Together with a global impairment in cognitive function, specific domains show greater dysfunction, such as episodic memory, working memory, and executive function (5, 6). There are also patients with schizophrenia with cognitive scores in the normal or above-normal range, varying in different studies from 15 to 45% (610). Nevertheless, in the high cognitive function subgroup, 64% still have abnormal scores on at least one cognitive domain, compared to 35% of healthy controls (7). Here a profile is considered abnormal if at least two functions are more than two SDs below the normative mean, or if only a single function is extremely impaired [i.e., >3 SDs below the normative mean (6)]. Therefore, most of the high functional group demonstrates some kind of cognitive deficit compared to healthy controls.

Investigating cognitive performance in patients at the early stages of the illness has the advantage of identifying cognitive deficits more likely to reflect the neurodysfunction that underlies schizophrenia rather than possible illness or treatment related processes following the chronic course of the illness. Of course, it is still possible that prodromal symptoms, or even very short periods of psychotic symptoms, lead to cognitive changes, but investigating patients at illness onset can give new insight into the disorder, when compared to the large amount of research that has been conducted in long-term ill patients. Moreover, these studies can also help elucidate whether or not some of the cognitive changes are a consequence of long-term pharmacological treatment. In considering studies in patients with first-episode psychosis (FEP), it is important to note that some have included not just patients with schizophrenia, but also those with other psychoses. This is relevant since during this early phase diagnosis may change (11), and not including such cases would mean losing potential schizophrenia cases. It can also elicit different cognitive profiles based on the heterogeneity of the first-episode group. For example, affective psychosis may have better cognitive profile than a non-affective schizophrenia (12).

Two recent articles have reviewed cognitive function during the course of schizophrenia and other psychosis (prodromal, first-episode, and chronic illness) (13, 14). However these have a very broad scope. Instead, we decided to carry out a systematic review of cognitive function in FEP; by focusing our review we were able to go into greater depth to elicit patterns of sub area of impairment measured by effect sizes. Compared to the review article by Mesholam-Gately et al. (15), effect sizes were calculated based on controls performance from the same catchment area as the patients, thus avoiding differences in possible sample group biases independent of psychosis. This is the first review article to present in detail effect sizes comparing (subtypes) of FEP and controls on cognitive performance, divided into domains. We will also include putative mechanisms behind these impairments, and both environmental and genetic factors will be discussed.

Only case-control studies were included for the neurocognitive meta-analysis.

Further, we have included all types of FEP, not only schizophrenia (15), giving the possibility to compare “first-episode schizophrenia” versus “other psychosis” on cognitive profile at start of their first-episode of psychosis. We will also investigate if underlying cognitive factors are driving the overall cognitive performance. Finally, we will discuss possible etiological aspects, specifically genetic and environmental factors, and how they interact.

Thus, the aim of this literature review is to present in detail effect sizes comparing FEP and controls from the same catchment area on cognitive performance divided into cognitive domains; 1) investigate and discuss if specific underlying cognitive factors are driving the overall cognitive function, as well as links to disease progression; 2) discuss putative mechanisms behind our findings including sensitivity to stress, and gene environmental interactions.

Methods

Cognitive Function in First-Episode Psychosis

A systematic review of the literature on cognition in FEP was conducted. Medline (PubMed) and PsycINFO bibliographic databases were used to search for articles reviewed. The search strategies involved the following keywords: “cognition and schizophrenia,” “cognitive function and schizophrenia,” cognitive function in FEP,” and “cognition in first- episode psychosis,” up to and including published papers June 2013. Criteria for inclusion were FEP, with cognitive measures (data on mean ± SD) for both patients and controls. Twenty-four articles fulfilled these criteria’s. The qualifying studies are presented in Tables 15, which include, for each cognitive domain area, the bibliographic reference, the cognitive tests used, and the degree of the impairment identified. Effect sizes were computed using Cohen’s d (16). According to Rosenthal and Rosnow (17), effect sizes were considered small for values between 0.20 and 0.50, moderate for values between 0.50 and 0.80, and large for values greater than 0.80. The effect sizes reflect those reported in the original published studies. For papers that did not report the effect size, the effect size was calculated on the basis of the mean and SD in the patient and the control groups.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Studies comparing patients with first-episode psychosis and healthy controls on general cognitive ability.

General Intellectual Function

General cognition is usually assessed in two ways. Firstly, general cognitive ability is measured by a set of multiple cognitive tests such as the Wechsler Adult Intelligence scale (WAIS). The WAIS is based on multiple performance and verbal cognitive tests, which together are believed to reflect general cognitive function. Secondly, general cognition is assessed with a single reading test, such as the National reading test (NART) (3) or the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading test (WTAR). The NART and the WTAR are reading tests which both estimate cognitive ability by reading a list of words, with the IQ level estimated on the basis of mistakes made in the pronunciation of the words listed (38). In the literature on FEP studies, the NART or the WTAR (or equivalent schedules) are normally used to assess pre-morbid intelligence, an estimate of intelligence level achieved before illness onset, while the WAIS is usually used as an estimate of current IQ (12).

Executive Function, Attention, and Working Memory

The term “executive functioning” is often used as a synonym for frontal lobe functioning, and to indicate higher cognitive functioning of the prefrontal cortex (2, 38). Executive function consists of loosely related higher-order cognitive processes, including: problem solving, planning, initiation, hypothesis generation, cognitive flexibility, decision making, regulation, and judgment and working memory (38). Working memory is conceived as a limited capacity storage for retaining information over a short time (1–2 min), and performing mental operations of the content during this period (38). Moreover, the context of the working memory may originate from both sensory inputs as well as from stored long-term memory (38). Attention can be defined as the gateway for information flow to the brain. Attention is a complex system that allows the individual to filter relevant and irrelevant information in the context of internal drives and intentions, hold and manipulate mental processes, and monitor responses to stimuli (38).

Memory

Memory is a complex process by which an individual registers retains and retrieves information (38). Memory is divided into non-verbal and verbal memory, with verbal memory including declarative memory and non-declarative memory. Declarative memory includes both episodic memory (memory for events) and semantic memory (memory for facts). Declarative memory includes an understanding of something being learned, while non-declarative memory can take place without conscious awareness (38).

Several tests are used to assess memory, and the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) Logical memory test has been widely used to assess declarative memory in psychosis. In the logical memory task, the participant is asked to recall a short story, immediately after hearing the story, and again after a delay of 30 min (38). Other widely used verbal memory tasks in psychosis research are the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) and the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), assessing memory functioning through the use of a word-list learning paradigm, which includes an immediate and delayed component similar to the Logical memory (38). Non-verbal memory is assessed by tests evaluating the ability to recall a specific visual design, including an immediate and delayed aspect, with Visual Reproduction and the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test being the most frequent tests used in the literature (38).

Processing Speed

Processing speed refers to the speed at which different cognitive tasks are completed. It normally involves a simple task, which is timed on completion. The importance of processing speed lies in its relevance to many higher cognitive operations, such as perceptual processes, encoding, and retrieval (38).

Motor Speed

Deficits in motor performance are seen in many neurological disturbances. The most frequently used motor tests in FEP are grooved pegboard and finger tapping. The grooved pegboard contains twenty-five holes with randomly positioned slots and pegs which have a key along one side. Pegs must be rotated to match the hole before they can be inserted as quickly as possible. For the finger tapping task the examinee is instructed to tap as rapidly as possible using index finger for a specific amount of time. For both grooved pegboard and finger tapping performance with the preferred hand usually demonstrates best performance (38).

Results

General Intellectual Function

Overall, patients with FEP scored significantly worse on current IQ assessed by the WAIS battery compared to controls (see Table 1). The largest effect size across FEP studies was observed for the WAIS full scale IQ (based on performance and verbal tasks from the WAIS), with an effect size of 1.71 (3). Significant differences between patients and controls have also been replicated in studies using NART or WTAR scores to measure pre-morbid IQ. Thus in our own work (21), we found a significant difference between NART scores in patients and controls, with an effect size of 1.09. Significant differences between patients and controls NART scores are also shown in other studies, with effect sizes varying between 0.39 and 1.05.

Executive Function, Attention, and Working Memory

Patients with FEP show impairment in executive performance, with effect sizes ranging from 0.25 to 1.86, with the most profound deficits observed Mohamed et al. (1) on Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) perseverative errors, with an effect size of 1.86 (see Table 2). Working memory follows with an effect size up to 1.42, followed by attention (effect size up to 1.30).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Studies comparing patients with first-episode psychosis and healthy controls on executive function, attention and working memory.

Memory

Several studies in FEP have shown significant impairments in memory, particularly in verbal declarative episodic memory (see Table 3). The effect sizes of verbal episodic memory deficits vary from 0.26 to 2.10, and the effect sizes for non-verbal memory vary from 0.38 to 1.65. As shown by the high effect size reported from studies on FEP, memory impairment is the most, or one of the most, impaired cognitive domains already at illness onset.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Studies comparing patients with first-episode psychosis and healthy controls on memory function.

Processing Speed

Studies of patients with FEP have shown reduced processing speed in these patients when compared to controls, with effect sizes ranging from 0.33 to 1.69 (see Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Studies comparing patients with first-episode psychosis and healthy controls on processing speed.

Motor Speed

Patients with FEP show impairments on motor speed (with effect sizes varying from 0.36 to 1.26; see Table 5).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Studies comparing patients with first-episode psychosis and healthy controls on motor speed.

Discussion

Reviewing the literature on cognitive function in FEP, we found significant impairment (Cohen’s d effect size above 0.8 (17) in all cognitive domains investigated; see Tables 15. The largest effect size was observed for verbal memory (Cohen’s d effect size = 2.10), followed by executive function (effect size = 1.86), and general IQ (effect size = 1.71). Our systematic review focused on FEP and included studies published up to 2013, thus covering studies not included previously in the review by Bozikas and Andreou (13) (included studies up to January 2010); or Lewandowski et al. (14) (included studies up to 31st of March 2010). Moreover, the study by Bozikas and Andreou (13) included only articles with longitudinal data, excluding studies presenting FEP only. Also the study by Lewandowski et al. (14) included articles on cognitive function in chronic schizophrenia and other psychosis. Our study therefore complements these articles, focusing in-depth on the first-episode only. We have also calculated effect sizes based on Cohen’s d, providing information about the severity of the impairment, which is not included in the previous reviews described above. Similar to the study of Bozikas and Andreou (13) and Lewandowski et al. (14) we found that already at illness onset patients with FEP are characterized by cognitive impairments across domains. Moreover, our results support different cognitive pattern for schizophrenia and affective psychosis (schizophrenia patients showing the greatest effect size differences in cognitive performance compared to controls). Further, we found more variation in the effect sizes comparing patients and controls on cognitive tests than the study by Reichenberg et al. (2) in chronic schizophrenia, indicating a further consolidation of cognitive impairment over time may be present. These results are similar to Bozikas and Andreou (13) and Lewandowski et al. (14) showing the main reduction in cognition around illness onset, or pre-morbid period, with a relatively stable course, which may become even more pronounced after some years.

Our findings in FEP demonstrate that already at illness onset patients have reduced IQ scores compared to controls. Both current IQ and estimated pre-morbid IQ were significantly lower than those of controls, with the greatest impairment in current IQ scores. Indeed, the discrepancy between current IQ and pre-morbid scores is believed to reflect the deterioration in current IQ at or around illness onset. The study by Zanelli et al. (12) show that when we examine schizophrenia versus other psychosis (psychosis NOS and affective psychosis), patients with schizophrenia have larger effect size differences in current IQ, as well as pre-morbid IQ (schizophrenia current IQ effect size = 1.52; pre-morbid IQ effect size = 0.79), compared to other psychosis (current IQ effect size = 0.53; pre-morbid IQ effect size = 0.45). This indicates worse cognitive function in the schizophrenia group both at illness onset and prior to illness onset, compared to patients with other psychosis. Patients in the schizophrenia group also present the greatest deterioration (measured by effect size) comparing current IQ to pre-morbid IQ [schizophrenia effect size current IQ (1.52) minus effect size pre-morbid IQ (0.79) = 0.73, compared to other psychosis: effect size current IQ (0.53) minus effect size pre-morbid IQ (0.45) = 0.08]. A worse cognitive profile in patients with schizophrenia was also found in the study by Hill et al. (39) showing that patients with FEP who developed schizophrenia demonstrated more impaired cognitive performance across domains, compared to patients with affective psychosis. Similar findings are also observed in chronic psychosis (14).

Several studies conducted in FEP have matched patients and controls on general cognition, in order to investigate specific deficits believed to be particularly marked in psychosis. It should be mentioned that matching subgroups on IQ aiming to measure specific cognitive areas believed to be impaired in psychosis may be challenging due to that such a comparison would be contaminated by regression toward the mean (Lord’s Paradox) (40).

Executive Function, Attention, and Working Memory

Although a strong correlation has been found between general intellectual function and other specific cognitive subareas, such as memory and executive functioning, research has also shown impairment of executive functions, such as planning, flexibility and judgment, without any major change in general intellectual status (38). Is the impairment in executive and working memory observed in patients with psychosis a consequence of general cognitive decline, or is executive impairment in psychosis independent from overall cognitive performance? Chan et al. (20) aimed to answer this question in a study of 78 patients with first-episode medication-naïve schizophrenia and 60 controls; they showed that patients with first-episode medication-naïve schizophrenia exhibit specific types of executive dysfunction independently of general cognitive decline. However, Table 2 shows that there is a great discrepancy across studies in terms of effect sizes observed; for example the effect size 0.22 was found for “Arithmetic” in the study by Leeson et al. (19), compared to an effect size of 0.77 in the study by Ma et al. (30). One difference between these two studies was that the study by Leeson et al. (19) had matched the patients and controls for general IQ, while the study by Ma et al. (30) had not. It is therefore important to have in mind whether general IQ has been taken into account in the analysis when interpreting effect sizes for different cognitive subtests.

Memory

It has been suggested that episodic memory tests could be used to elicit diagnostic differences in a psychosis sample. For example, in the study by Fitzgerald et al. (41) consisting of 83 patients with FEP, decreased performance on verbal memory was reported in patients with schizophrenia compared to patients with affective psychosis, implying diagnostic differences in performance on this specific memory task. Prospective studies also show worse cognitive profile in patients with FEP who later develop schizophrenia across cognitive domains, compared to patients with affective psychosis (12, 39).

Processing Speed

The study by Leeson et al. (19), comparing FEP and healthy controls (matched for general cognition) reported that processing speed was the cognitive domain most impaired in FEP. Leeson et al. (19) also found a relationship between processing speed performance and performance on executive and memory tasks, two known impaired cognitive domains in psychosis, as discussed earlier. This indicates the importance of measuring processing speed when studying patients with psychosis. As processing speed is important for the performance of higher cognitive operations, such as executive functions and memory, it has been suggested that impaired processing speed underlies the abnormalities in these areas (42). On the other hand, to complicate this further, a reduction of processing speed has also been found associated with antipsychotic medication (43).

Below are hypothesis about putative mechanism(s) of the cognitive reduction in FEP, investigating the role of early stress, inflammation and genetic vulnerability.

Stress and Cognition in Psychosis

Some previous studies in chronic patients with psychosis, and in healthy controls, report a negative correlation between childhood trauma and cognitive function, particularly decreased scores on general cognitive abilities, memory, and executive function (4448). This is interesting as similar cognitive areas are found to be most reduced in FEP, comparing patients to controls performance (see Result). The deleterious effect of stress and glucocorticoid on the brain (particularly on hippocampus) is well known in the literature (49, 50), and indeed disorders characterized by increased stress exposure such as depression, post-traumatic stress disorder, chronic fatigue disorder, show cognitive impairment, particularly in memory and in executive function (5153). Moreover, psychosis is a disorder characterized by high level of stressful events (54), including recent stressful events (55), the inability to cope with life events (56, 57), and childhood trauma (58, 59). It is therefore possible that some of the cognitive impairment in FEP is due to an increased exposure, or vulnerability toward the negative effect of particularly childhood trauma. We will now review the literature supporting this putative mechanism.

Cortisol Level and Cognitive Function and Psychosis

Over two decades have past since the first study investigating Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal (HPA) axis and cognitive abnormalities in schizophrenia were published (60) and suggested a link between hypercortisolism and cognitive impairment. Since then, several studies have investigated HPA axis and cognitive function in long-term chronic schizophrenic patients. The first study by Saffer et al. (60) consisted of 11 patients with type II schizophrenia (mainly negative symptoms), 34 controls, 30 patients with schizophrenia with type 1 (mainly positive symptoms), and 9 patients with mixed type I and type II schizophrenia (both negative and positive symptoms). This study found a strong negative correlation between dexamethasone abnormalities (dexamethasone non-suppressors) and cognitive performance in type II schizophrenics, implying a relationship between HPA axis hyperactivity and cognitive impairment. Furthermore, Walder et al. (61), evaluating 18 patients with chronic psychosis, 7 patients without psychosis, and 19 healthy controls, found that, in the entire sample, cortisol levels were negatively correlated with performance on memory and executive tasks.

Also in FEP an abnormal HPA axis has been demonstrated (55) showing higher cortisol levels during the day in the patients compared to the controls. An interesting closed-loop model, first hypothesized by Lupien (62), gives a possible explanation for the relationship between cortisol level and cognition in schizophrenia. The closed-loop model works in the following manner: basal cortisol level increases symptoms severity, which again impacts on the encoding of incoming information into long-term memory store; this leads to difficulties discriminating between relevant (threat) and irrelevant (non-threat) information, which again further increases the reactivity to stress.

In our recent paper on patients with FEP (21), we found that patients performed significantly worse on all cognitive domains compared to controls. In patients only, a blunted cortisol awakening response (that is, more abnormal) was associated with a more severe deficit in verbal memory and processing speed, supporting a role for the HPA axis, as measured by cortisol awakening response, in modulating cognitive function in patients with psychosis.

Childhood Trauma and Cognitive Function in Psychosis

Six studies in the literature have investigated cognition and early trauma in people with psychosis, four in individuals including chronic illness phase. The first study by Lysaker et al. (45) was conducted in males with schizophrenia spectrum disorder, and no comparison control group was used. The authors found that male patients with childhood trauma (sexual abuse) had impaired processing speed, working memory, and executive function compared with patients without abuse (45). The second study by Schenkel et al. (47) included 15 female and 25 males with schizophrenia spectrum disorder, with no comparison control group. The authors found that patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder with a history of childhood trauma showed a decreased score on learning and visual context processing compared to non-abused patients. Similar findings were found in the study by Shannon et al. (48) in a sample of 85 patients (67 males and 18 females) with chronic schizophrenia; here patients with childhood trauma scored significantly lower on working memory as well as verbal memory tasks, even after controlling for pre-morbid IQ and depressive symptoms. Similar findings were observed in our own examination of the large AESOP study of FEP which comprised of 138 patients (73 males and 65 females) and 138 controls (64 males and 74 females), showing an association between childhood trauma and reduced performance on attention, concentration and mental speed, language, and verbal intelligence; however this was mainly driven by male patients with affective psychosis (44), whilst in controls only one subtest (performance intelligence) was reduced in the group with high exposure of childhood trauma, and in males only. Also in an independent study including 406 patients with a mean duration of illness of 3 years showed a strong negative correlation between cognitive function and a history of childhood trauma; here childhood trauma was associated with a reduction in cognitive function across cognitive domains in patients with schizophrenia spectrum- and bipolar disorders, in particular working memory and executive function, as well as general cognition. Moreover, these dysfunctions were driven by underlying deficits in general cognitive tasks as measured by the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) (63). Only two studies have found negative, or inconclusive, findings between cognitive function and early trauma in FEP (21, 64), with the latter and largest study showing an effect of childhood trauma on cognitive function in controls, but not in patients (64).

Potential Biological Mechanisms

Since patients with psychotic disorders – in addition to being more exposed to trauma – also demonstrate abnormalities in the HPA axis (55), immunological disturbances (65, 66), together with cognitive and brain structure abnormalities (6, 12, 21, 67), it is of interest to investigate the link between stress exposure and biological stress parameters to cognitive abnormalities in psychosis. Based on the findings outlined above, it is particularly intriguing to investigate to what extent patients with psychotic disorders also have a genetic vulnerability toward abnormal stress response that may add to – or interact with the effects of early trauma, and cognitive impairments.

There are several indications of an abnormal HPA axis in patients with psychotic disorders irrespective of early trauma. This includes findings of an increased pituitary volume in patients with FEP compared to controls (68, 69). There are also signs of an altered or increased systemic cortisol metabolism (7072) with links to genetic markers in psychosis (71), which might interact with the effect of stressful life event. The idea of a genetic predisposition to an abnormal stress response is supported by indications that not only patients with psychosis have abnormal stress responses, but also their relatives (73). Moreover, increased cortisol levels correlate with smaller hippocampal volume in psychosis (74). There is also an interaction between glucocorticoids and serotonin in the central nervous system (CNS) as glucocorticoids regulate both tryptophan hydroxylase and the expression of several serotonin receptors (75). In addition, the functional polymorphism in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR) has been linked to altered stress response, as carriers of the short (s-) allele have increased negative psychological reactions and stress hormone release compared with carriers of the long (l-) allele (76). Indeed, our study Aas et al. (76) demonstrated that patients with an early psychotic illness who were carriers of the short (s-) allele of the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), exposed to high levels of childhood trauma (physical neglect and abuse) had significantly poorer cognitive functioning, than all other groups. Patients with psychosis also show reduced brain-derived neurotropic factor (BDNF) levels both in the brain (77), and in serum and plasma (7779), which may be related to their cognitive performance (80). The BDNF gene has at least one functional variant, the SNP (rs 6265) resulting in a Valine (val) to Methionine (met) substitution at codon 66 of the proBDNF. The low active met allele is here related to reductions in BDNF release (81). Recent research in FEP have found associations between reduced BDNF gene expression levels, childhood trauma, increased inflammation, and smaller hippocampal volume (82). Reduced BDNF levels are also observed in bipolar patients exposed to childhood trauma (83), as well as linked to cognition in animals (84). It is well known that patients with psychosis show brain structural abnormalities compared to healthy controls (67, 85, 86). A recent meta-analyses in healthy individuals indicate hippocampus volume reductions in met carriers compared to homozygotic val carriers (87); this is an area of the brain important for memory. In psychosis, met carriers usually demonstrate larger ventricles, more CSF, and reduced frontal gray matter volume (88, 89), even if there are inconsistent results (90). While the study by Mondelli et al. (82) did not genotype, their study support the role of BDNF on brain structures, as well as a relationship between childhood trauma and BDNF levels. Childhood trauma may thus represent a significant factor influencing cognitive function in psychosis, mediated through an effect on BDNF. Indeed our recent study in patients with a psychotic illness (80) support this hypothesis, showing that BDNF val66met-met carriers reporting high levels of childhood trauma demonstrate significantly reduced executive function/working memory as well as smaller hippocampal volume, compared to all other groups.

Several lines of evidence have implicated the immune system in the development of severe psychiatric disorders, and increased inflammation is found in depression (9193), as well as in psychosis (65, 66, 94). Recent GWAS studies clearly indicate immune genes as susceptibility genes for schizophrenia (92, 95). Increased inflammation is also found to be associated with cognitive impairments in animals (96). Pilot data from a small sample of patients with FEP and healthy controls show a relationship between increased inflammation parameters (IL-8 and IL-6) and cognitive impairments across both groups (97), indicating that immune markers may be related to cognitive function in psychosis. More studies should investigate this further, aiming to understand the cognitive reduction seen in FEP compared to healthy controls.

Epigenetic Research Related to Schizophrenia Risk and Cognitive Function

Recent epigenetic research related to schizophrenia risk, links early stress (hypoxia and perinatal stress) to methylation and altered gene expression associated with behavior and cognitive changes (98). Perinatal stress has also been found to be related to reduction of Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in the brain of adult rats (84). Riva et al. (84) also show that perinatal stress in male rats is related to reduced performance on the object recognition test in the adult rat. Riva linked the above to epigenetic changes, explaining that perinatal stress increases methylation, which again is related to a reduced transcript of BDNF, and cognitive changes. Riva et al. (84) has also demonstrated that perinatal stress is associated with reduced upregulation of BDNF on the forced swim test in adult rats, linking this to impaired ability to cope under stressful situations in rats exposed to perinatal stress. Finally, Riva et al. (84) also found that epigenetic changes in response to stress are seen across genes: methylome analysis in the prefrontal cortex of rats exposed to perinatal stress showed that a large number of genes (3,386 genes) were methylated differently from controls. These included genes linked to bipolar disorders and schizophrenia, such as CACNA1C and DISC1, as well as the COMT gene. Further research should therefore investigate links between early stress, and epigenetic changes of relevant genes across genome, aiming to understand mechanisms behind cognitive abnormalities in patients with FEP.

Limitations

This study has several limitations which should be acknowledged: we decided to focus on only some aspects which may influence cognitive function (i.e., stress, and interactions between stress and genetic factors, as well as inflammation), and we did not have the possibility to go into depth on other important aspects, such as time of illness onset (early illness related to worse cognitive function), possible gender differences (males lower performance), the influence of antipsychotics and medication, and the duration of treatment. Although all these patients are FEP, the exact level of treatment varied between the studies (see Supplementary Material for an overview of recruitment in the different papers discussed). Moreover other environmental factors such as cannabis have both been associated with increased risk of the illness, as well as influencing cognitive function, however due to space and focus of the paper, we focused on the role of stress and inflammation and genetics related to these factors as possible explanation model. Only the Cohen’s d and not the confidence intervals for the Cohen’s d are included in this study. However, we have included in the Supplementary Material a detailed overview of the studies included aiming to facilitate interpretations of the findings. As already mentioned matching subgroups on IQ aiming to measure specific cognitive areas believed to be impaired in psychosis may also be challenging due to that such a comparison may be influenced by regression toward the mean (Lord’s Paradox) (40).

Summary

We have demonstrated that cognitive impairment across domains (up to and above severe level based on Cohen’s d effect size) is present already at illness onset, as shown in FEP studies. However, differences in levels of impairment are observed between studies, as well as within domains, indicating that compared to chronic schizophrenia (2), a further consolidation of cognitive impairment over time may be present.

The research into trauma, stress, and HPA axis disturbances (including gene interactions) is one of the areas where we have some knowledge about how specific environment and genetic factors influence cognition (Figure 1). Indeed, early stress may have long-lasting changes on cognitive function by affecting expression of relevant genes. This is the start of a new line of research aimed at further understanding the complex etiology behind cognitive abnormalities in psychosis. Based on published biological studies, we propose that some of these impairments may be due to these subjects’ increased sensitivity to the negative effect of stress, and genetic vulnerability, affecting the HPA axis, the immune function, and neuroplasticity. More studies are needed aiming to understand the complex etiology of cognitive impairments in psychosis.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Suggests that cognitive impairment in FEP is related to neurodevelopmental abnormalities. Figure 1 also postulate that stress, as well as genetic vulnerability may be part of the complex etiology behind cognitive impairment in psychosis.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Some of the work leading to this review has been funded by the British Academy and by the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust & Institute of Psychiatry NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health. This study was also funded by grants from the University of Oslo, South-Eastern Norway Health Authority (#2004123, #2006258) and the Research Council of Norway (#167153/V50, #163070/V50, #190311/V50).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at http://www.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00182/abstract

References

1. Mohamed S, Paulsen JS, O’Leary D, Arndt S, Andreasen N. Generalized cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a study of first-episode patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1999) 56:749–54. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.8.749

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

2. Reichenberg A, Harvey PD. Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia: integration of performance-based and brain imaging findings. Psychol Bull (2007) 133:833–58. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.833

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

3. Bilder RM, Goldman RS, Robinson D, Reiter G, Bell L, Bates JA, et al. Neuropsychology of first-episode schizophrenia: initial characterization and clinical correlates. Am J Psychiatry (2000) 157:549–59. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.4.549

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

4. Hoff AL, Riordan H, O’Donnell DW, Morris L, DeLisi LE. Neuropsychological functioning of first-episode schizophreniform patients. Am J Psychiatry (1992) 149:898–903.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

5. Flashman LA, Green MF. Review of cognition and brain structure in schizophrenia: profiles, longitudinal course, and effects of treatment. Psychiatr Clin North Am (2004) 27:1–18. doi:10.1016/S0193-953X(03)00105-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

6. Reichenberg A, Harvey PD, Bowie CR, Mojtabai R, Rabinowitz J, Heaton RK, et al. Neuropsychological function and dysfunction in schizophrenia and psychotic affective disorders. Schizophr Bull (2009) 35:1022–9. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn044

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

7. Allen DN, Goldstein G, Warnick E. A consideration of neuropsychologically normal schizophrenia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2003) 9:56–63.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

8. Goldstein G. Neuropsychological heterogeneity in schizophrenia: a consideration of abstraction and problem-solving abilities. Arch Clin Neuropsychol (1990) 5:251–64. doi:10.1093/arclin/5.3.251

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

9. Heinrichs RW, Awad AG. Neurocognitive subtypes of chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (1993) 9:49–58. doi:10.1016/0920-9964(93)90009-8

CrossRef Full Text

10. Silverstein ML, Zerwic MJ. Clinical psychopathologic symptoms in neuropsychologically impaired and intact schizophrenics. J Consult Clin Psychol (1985) 53:267–8. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.53.2.267

CrossRef Full Text

11. Amin S, Singh SP, Brewin J, Jones PB, Medley I, Harrison G. Diagnostic stability of first-episode psychosis. Comparison of ICD-10 and DSM-III-R systems. Br J Psychiatry (1999) 175:537–43. doi:10.1192/bjp.175.6.537

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

12. Zanelli J, Reichenberg A, Morgan K, Fearon P, Kravariti E, Dazzan P, et al. Specific and generalized neuropsychological deficits: a comparison of patients with various first-episode psychosis presentations. Am J Psychiatry (2010) 167:78–85. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09010118

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

13. Bozikas VP, Andreou C. Longitudinal studies of cognition in first episode psychosis: a systematic review of the literature. Aust N Z J Psychiatry (2011) 45:93–108. doi:10.3109/00048674.2010.541418

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

14. Lewandowski KE, Cohen BM, Ongur D. Evolution of neuropsychological dysfunction during the course of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Psychol Med (2011) 41:225–41. doi:10.1017/S0033291710001042

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

15. Mesholam-Gately RI, Giuliano AJ, Goff KP, Faraone SV, Seidman LJ. Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology (2009) 23:315–36. doi:10.1037/a0014708

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

16. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: Academic Press (1977).

17. Rosenthal R, Rosnow RL. Essentials of Behavioural Research: Methods and Data Analysis. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill (1984).

18. Hill SK, Beers SR, Kmiec JA, Keshavan MS, Sweeney JA. Impairment of verbal memory and learning in antipsychotic-naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2004) 68:127–36. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00125-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

19. Leeson VC, Barnes TR, Harrison M, Matheson E, Harrison I, Mutsatsa SH, et al. The relationship between IQ, memory, executive function, and processing speed in recent-onset psychosis: 1-year stability and clinical outcome. Schizophr Bull (2010) 36:400–9. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn100

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

20. Chan RC, Chen EY, Law CW. Specific executive dysfunction in patients with first-episode medication-naive schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2006) 82:51–64. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2005.09.020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

21. Aas M, Dazzan P, Mondelli V, Toulopoulou T, Reichenberg A, Di FM, et al. Abnormal cortisol awakening response predicts worse cognitive function in patients with first-episode psychosis. Psychol Med (2011) 41:463–76. doi:10.1017/S0033291710001170

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

22. Joyce E, Hutton S, Mutsatsa S, Gibbins H, Webb E, Paul S, et al. Executive dysfunction in first-episode schizophrenia and relationship to duration of untreated psychosis: the West London Study. Br J Psychiatry Suppl (2002) 43:s38–44. doi:10.1192/bjp.181.43.s38

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

23. Mathes B, Wood SJ, Proffitt TM, Stuart GW, Buchanan JA, Velakoulis D, et al. Early processing deficits in object working memory in first-episode schizophreniform psychosis and established schizophrenia. Psychol Med (2005) 35:1053–62. doi:10.1017/S0033291705004617

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

24. Hermens DF, Ward PB, Hodge MA, Kaur M, Naismith SL, Hickie IB. Impaired MMN/P3a complex in first-episode psychosis: cognitive and psychosocial associations. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2010) 34:822–9. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.03.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

25. Rodriguez-Sanchez JM, Crespo-Facorro B, Gonzalez-Blanch C, Perez-Iglesias R, Vazquez-Barquero JL. Cognitive dysfunction in first-episode psychosis: the processing speed hypothesis. Br J Psychiatry Suppl (2007) 51:s107–10. doi:10.1192/bjp.191.51.s107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

26. Ngoma M, Vansteelandt K, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L, Miezi SM, Peuskens J. Cognitive deficits in nonaffective functional psychoses: a study in the Democratic Republic of Congo. Psychiatry Res (2010) 180:86–92. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2009.10.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

27. Zabala A, Rapado M, Arango C, Robles O, de la Serna E, Gonzalez C, et al. Neuropsychological functioning in early-onset first-episode psychosis: comparison of diagnostic subgroups. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2010) 260:225–33. doi:10.1007/s00406-009-0046-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

28. Addington J, Addington D. Social and cognitive functioning in psychosis. Schizophr Res (2008) 99:176–81. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.07.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

29. Addington J, Brooks BL, Addington D. Cognitive functioning in first episode psychosis: initial presentation. Schizophr Res (2003) 62:59–64. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00340-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

30. Ma X, Wang Q, Sham PC, Liu X, Rabe-Hesketh S, Sun X, et al. Neurocognitive deficits in first-episode schizophrenic patients and their first-degree relatives. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet (2007) 144B:407–16. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.30330

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

31. Pena J, Ojeda N, Segarra R, Eguiluz JI, Garcia J, Gutierrez M. Executive functioning correctly classified diagnoses in patients with first-episode psychosis: evidence from a 2-year longitudinal study. Schizophr Res (2011) 126:77–80. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.09.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

32. Brickman AM, Buchsbaum MS, Bloom R, Bokhoven P, Paul-Odouard R, Haznedar MM, et al. Neuropsychological functioning in first-break, never-medicated adolescents with psychosis. J Nerv Ment Dis (2004) 192:615–22. doi:10.1097/01.nmd.0000138229.29157.3e

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

33. Perez-Iglesias R, Tordesillas-Gutierrez D, McGuire PK, Barker GJ, Roiz-Santianez R, Mata I, et al. White matter integrity and cognitive impairment in first-episode psychosis. Am J Psychiatry (2010) 167:451–8. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09050716

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

34. Gonzalez-Blanch C, Perez-Iglesias R, Rodriguez-Sanchez JM, Pardo-Garcia G, Martinez-Garcia O, Vazquez-Barquero JL, et al. A digit symbol coding task as a screening instrument for cognitive impairment in first-episode psychosis. Arch Clin Neuropsychol (2011) 26:48–58. doi:10.1093/arclin/acq086

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

35. Gonzalez-Blanch C, varez-Jimenez M, Rodriguez-Sanchez JM, Perez-Iglesias R, Vazquez-Barquero JL, Crespo-Facorro B. Cognitive functioning in the early course of first-episode schizophrenia spectrum disorders: timing and patterns. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2006) 256:364–71. doi:10.1007/s00406-006-0646-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

36. Liu KC, Chan RC, Chan KK, Tang JY, Chiu CP, Lam MM, et al. Executive function in first-episode schizophrenia: a three-year longitudinal study of an ecologically valid test. Schizophr Res (2011) 126:87–92. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.11.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

37. Kleinlogel H, Strik W, Begre S. Increased NoGo-anteriorisation in first-episode schizophrenia patients during Continuous Performance Test. Clin Neurophysiol (2007) 118:2683–91. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2007.08.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

38. Spreen OSE. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. Oxford University Press (1998).

39. Hill SK, Reilly JL, Harris MS, Rosen C, Marvin RW, Deleon O, et al. A comparison of neuropsychological dysfunction in first-episode psychosis patients with unipolar depression, bipolar disorder, and schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2009) 113:167–75. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2009.04.020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

40. Miller GA, Chapman JP. Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. J Abnorm Psychol (2001) 110:40–8. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.110.1.40

CrossRef Full Text

41. Fitzgerald D, Lucas S, Redoblado MA, Winter V, Brennan J, Anderson J, et al. Cognitive functioning in young people with first episode psychosis: relationship to diagnosis and clinical characteristics. Aust N Z J Psychiatry (2004) 38:501–10. doi:10.1080/j.1440-1614.2004.01403.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

42. Dickinson D, Ramsey ME, Gold JM. Overlooking the obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2007) 64:532–42. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.532

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

43. Veselinovic T, Schorn H, Vernaleken IB, Hiemke C, Zernig G, Gur R, et al. Effects of antipsychotic treatment on cognition in healthy subjects. J Psychopharmacol (2013) 27:374–85. doi:10.1177/0269881112466183

CrossRef Full Text

44. Aas M, Dazzan P, Fisher HL, Morgan C, Morgan K, Reichenberg A, et al. Childhood trauma and cognitive function in first-episode affective and non-affective psychosis. Schizophr Res (2011) 129:12–9. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2011.03.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

45. Lysaker PH, Meyer P, Evans JD, Marks KA. Neurocognitive and symptom correlates of self-reported childhood sexual abuse in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Ann Clin Psychiatry (2001) 13:89–92. doi:10.3109/10401230109148953

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

46. Perez CM, Widom CS. Childhood victimization and long-term intellectual and academic outcomes. Child Abuse Negl (1994) 18:617–33. doi:10.1016/0145-2134(94)90012-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

47. Schenkel LS, Spaulding WD, DiLillo D, Silverstein SM. Histories of childhood maltreatment in schizophrenia: relationships with premorbid functioning, symptomatology, and cognitive deficits. Schizophr Res (2005) 76:273–86. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2005.03.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

48. Shannon C, Douse K, McCusker C, Feeney L, Barrett S, Mulholland C. The association between childhood trauma and memory functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull (2011) 37:531–7. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbp096

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

49. Sapolsky RM, Krey LC, McEwen BS. The neuroendocrinology of stress and aging: the glucocorticoid cascade hypothesis. Endocr Rev (1986) 7:284–301. doi:10.1210/edrv-7-3-284

CrossRef Full Text

50. Sapolsky RM. The possibility of neurotoxicity in the hippocampus in major depression: a primer on neuron death. Biol Psychiatry (2000) 48:755–65. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00971-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

51. Porter RJ, Gallagher P, Thompson JM, Young AH. Neurocognitive impairment in drug-free patients with major depressive disorder. Br J Psychiatry (2003) 182:214–20. doi:10.1192/bjp.182.3.214

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

52. Sandstrom A, Rhodin IN, Lundberg M, Olsson T, Nyberg L. Impaired cognitive performance in patients with chronic burnout syndrome. Biol Psychol (2005) 69:271–9. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.08.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

53. Weber DL, Clark CR, McFarlane AC, Moores KA, Morris P, Egan GF. Abnormal frontal and parietal activity during working memory updating in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res (2005) 140:27–44. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2005.07.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

54. Brown GW, Birley JL. Crises and life changes and the onset of schizophrenia. J Health Soc Behav (1968) 9:203–14. doi:10.2307/2948405

CrossRef Full Text

55. Mondelli V, Dazzan P, Hepgul N, Di FM, Aas M, D’Albenzio A, et al. Abnormal cortisol levels during the day and cortisol awakening response in first-episode psychosis: the role of stress and of antipsychotic treatment. Schizophr Res (2010) 116:234–42. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2009.08.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

56. Horan WP, Ventura J, Nuechterlein KH, Subotnik KL, Hwang SS, Mintz J. Stressful life events in recent-onset schizophrenia: reduced frequencies and altered subjective appraisals. Schizophr Res (2005) 75:363–74. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2004.07.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

57. Zubin J, Spring B. Vulnerability – a new view of schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol (1977) 86:103–26. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.86.2.103

CrossRef Full Text

58. Fisher HL, Craig TK, Fearon P, Morgan K, Dazzan P, Lappin J, et al. Reliability and comparability of psychosis patients’ retrospective reports of childhood abuse. Schizophr Bull (2011) 37:546–53. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbp103

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

59. Fisher HL, Jones PB, Fearon P, Craig TK, Dazzan P, Morgan K, et al. The varying impact of type, timing and frequency of exposure to childhood adversity on its association with adult psychotic disorder. Psychol Med (2010) 40(12):1967–78. doi:10.1017/S0033291710000231

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

60. Saffer D, Metcalfe M, Coppen A. Abnormal dexamethasone suppression test in type II schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry (1985) 147:721–3. doi:10.1192/bjp.147.6.721

CrossRef Full Text

61. Walder DJ, Walker EF, Lewine RJ. Cognitive functioning, cortisol release, and symptom severity in patients with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2000) 48:1121–32. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01052-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

62. Lupien SJ. Stress and schizophrenia: the importance of cognition. Biol Psychiatry (2000) 48:1119–20. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01079-9

CrossRef Full Text

63. Aas M, Steen NE, Agartz I, Aminoff SR, Lorentzen S, Sundet K, et al. Is cognitive impairment following early life stress in severe mental disorders based on specific or general cognitive functioning? Psychiatry Res (2012) 198:495–500. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.045

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

64. Sideli L, Fisher HL, Russo M, Murray RM, Stilo SA, Wiffen BD, et al. Failure to find association between childhood abuse and cognition in first-episode psychosis patients. Eur Psychiatry (2013). doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2013.02.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

65. Dieset I, Hope S, Ueland T, Bjella T, Agartz I, Melle I, et al. Cardiovascular risk factors during second generation antipsychotic treatment are associated with increased C-reactive protein. Schizophr Res (2012) 140:169–74. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.06.040

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

66. Hope S, Melle I, Aukrust P, Steen NE, Birkenaes AB, Lorentzen S, et al. Similar immune profile in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: selective increase in soluble tumor necrosis factor receptor I and von Willebrand factor. Bipolar Disord (2009) 11:726–34. doi:10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00757.x

CrossRef Full Text

67. Steen RG, Mull C, McClure R, Hamer RM, Lieberman JA. Brain volume in first-episode schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging studies. Br J Psychiatry (2006) 188:510–8. doi:10.1192/bjp.188.6.510

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

68. Pariante CM, Vassilopoulou K, Velakoulis D, Phillips L, Soulsby B, Wood SJ, et al. Pituitary volume in psychosis. Br J Psychiatry (2004) 185:5–10. doi:10.1192/bjp.185.1.5

CrossRef Full Text

69. Pariante CM. Pituitary volume in psychosis: the first review of the evidence. J Psychopharmacol (2008) 22:76–81. doi:10.1177/0269881107084020

CrossRef Full Text

70. Paterson JM, Holmes MC, Kenyon CJ, Carter R, Mullins JJ, Seckl JR. Liver-selective transgene rescue of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysfunction in 11beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 1-deficient mice. Endocrinology (2007) 148:961–6. doi:10.1210/en.2006-0603

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

71. Steen NE, Tesli M, Kahler AK, Methlie P, Hope S, Barrett EA, et al. SRD5A2 is associated with increased cortisol metabolism in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2010) 34(8):1500–6. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.08.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

72. Steen NE, Methlie P, Lorentzen S, Hope S, Barrett EA, Larsson S, et al. Increased systemic cortisol metabolism in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a mechanism for increased stress vulnerability? J Clin Psychiatry (2011) 72:1515–21. doi:10.4088/JCP.10m06068yel

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

73. Mondelli V, Dazzan P, Gabilondo A, Tournikioti K, Walshe M, Marshall N, et al. Pituitary volume in unaffected relatives of patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2008) 33:1004–12. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.05.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

74. Mondelli V, Pariante CM, Navari S, Aas M, D’Albenzio A, Di FM, et al. Higher cortisol levels are associated with smaller left hippocampal volume in first-episode psychosis. Schizophr Res (2010) 119:75–8. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2009.12.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

75. Herbert J, Goodyer IM, Grossman AB, Hastings MH, de Kloet ER, Lightman SL, et al. Do corticosteroids damage the brain? J Neuroendocrinol (2006) 18:393–411. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2826.2006.01429.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

76. Aas M, Djurovic S, Athanasiu L, Steen NE, Agartz I, Lorentzen S, et al. Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism, childhood trauma, and cognition in patients with psychotic disorders. Schizophr Bull (2012) 38:15–22. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbr113

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

77. Durany N, Michel T, Zochling R, Boissl KW, Cruz-Sanchez FF, Riederer P, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and neurotrophin 3 in schizophrenic psychoses. Schizophr Res (2001) 52:79–86. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00084-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

78. Buckley PF, Pillai A, Evans D, Stirewalt E, Mahadik S. Brain derived neurotropic factor in first-episode psychosis. Schizophr Res (2007) 91:1–5. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.12.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

79. Ikeda Y, Yahata N, Ito I, Nagano M, Toyota T, Yoshikawa T, et al. Low serum levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor and epidermal growth factor in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2008) 101:58–66. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2008.01.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

80. Aas M, Haukvik UK, Djurovic S, Bergmann O, Athanasiu L, Tesli MS, et al. BDNF val66met modulates the association between childhood trauma, cognitive and brain abnormalities in psychoses. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2013) 46:181–8. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.07.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

81. Egan MF, Kojima M, Callicott JH, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, Bertolino A, et al. The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell (2003) 112:257–69. doi:10.1016/S0092-8674(03)00035-7

CrossRef Full Text

82. Mondelli V, Cattaneo A, Belvederi MM, Di FM, Handley R, Hepgul N, et al. Stress and inflammation reduce brain-derived neurotrophic factor expression in first-episode psychosis: a pathway to smaller hippocampal volume. J Clin Psychiatry (2011) 72:1677–84. doi:10.4088/JCP.10m06745

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

83. Kauer-Sant’Anna M, Tramontina J, Andreazza AC, Cereser K, da Costa S, Santin A, et al. Traumatic life events in bipolar disorder: impact on BDNF levels and psychopathology. Bipolar Disord (2007) 9(Suppl 1):128–35. doi:10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00478.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

84. Riva M. Adverse life events during gestation and long-term brain dysfunction: molecular and epigenetic mechanisms. 4th International Congress On Schizophrenia Research, Orlando (2013).

85. Hulshoff Pol HE, Schnack HG, Bertens MG, van Haren NE, van der Tweel I, Staal WG, et al. Volume changes in gray matter in patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2002) 159:244–50. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.2.244

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

86. Rimol LM, Hartberg CB, Nesvag R, Fennema-Notestine C, Hagler DJ Jr, Pung CJ, et al. Cortical thickness and subcortical volumes in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry (2010) 68:41–50. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.03.036

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

87. Hajek T, Kopecek M, Hoschl C. Reduced hippocampal volumes in healthy carriers of brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism: meta-analysis. World J Biol Psychiatry (2012) 13:178–87. doi:10.3109/15622975.2011.580005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

88. Chepenik LG, Fredericks C, Papademetris X, Spencer L, Lacadie C, Wang F, et al. Effects of the brain-derived neurotrophic growth factor val66met variation on hippocampus morphology in bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology (2009) 34:944–51. doi:10.1038/npp.2008.107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

89. Ho BC, Andreasen NC, Dawson JD, Wassink TH. Association between brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met gene polymorphism and progressive brain volume changes in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2007) 164:1890–9. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.05111903

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

90. Smith GN, Thornton AE, Lang DJ, Macewan GW, Ehmann TS, Kopala LC, et al. Hippocampal volume and the brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism in first episode psychosis. Schizophr Res (2012) 134:253–9. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2011.11.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

91. Leonard BE. The immune system, depression and the action of antidepressants. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2001) 25:767–80. doi:10.1016/S0278-5846(01)00155-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

92. Stefansson H, Ophoff RA, Steinberg S, Andreassen OA, Cichon S, Rujescu D, et al. Common variants conferring risk of schizophrenia. Nature (2009) 460:744–7. doi:10.1038/nature08186

CrossRef Full Text

93. Zunszain PA, Anacker C, Cattaneo A, Carvalho LA, Pariante CM. Glucocorticoids, cytokines and brain abnormalities in depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2011) 35(3):722–9. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.04.011

CrossRef Full Text

94. Di NM, Cattaneo A, Hepgul N, Di FM, Aitchison KJ, Janiri L, et al. Serum and gene expression profile of cytokines in first-episode psychosis. Brain Behav Immun (2013) 31:90–5. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2012.06.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

95. Ripke S, Sanders AR, Kendler KS, Levinson DF, Sklar P, Holmans PA, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies five new schizophrenia loci. Nat Genet (2011) 43:969–76. doi:10.1038/ng.940

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

96. Cunningham C, Campion S, Lunnon K, Murray CL, Woods JF, Deacon RM, et al. Systemic inflammation induces acute behavioral and cognitive changes and accelerates neurodegenerative disease. Biol Psychiatry (2009) 65:304–12. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.07.024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

97. Aas M, Mondelli V, Toulopoulou T, Reichenberg A, Di Forti M, Marques T, et al. Is inflammation linked to cognitive impairment in first-episode psychosis and in healthy controls? 17th Annual Meeting of The PsychoNeuroImmunology Research Society: Crossing Disciplines to Combat Disease, Dublin (2010).

98. Bertolino A. Hypoxia–related methylation of the BDNF Rs6265 Val allele differentially predicts risk for phenotypes related to schizophrenia in homozygous and heterozygous individuals. 14th International Congress On Schizophrenia Research, Orlando (2013).

Keywords: cognition, first-episode psychosis, stress, physiological, genes × environment interactions, review of literature

Citation: Aas M, Dazzan P, Mondelli V, Melle I, Murray RM and Pariante CM (2014) A systematic review of cognitive function in first-episode psychosis, including a discussion on childhood trauma, stress, and inflammation. Front. Psychiatry 4:182. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00182

Received: 20 August 2013; Accepted: 13 December 2013;
Published online: 08 January 2014.

Edited by:

Bruno Etain, Assistance Publique des Hopitaux de Paris et INSERM, France

Reviewed by:

Benedicto Crespo-Facorro, Universitario Hospital Marques de Valdecilla, Spain; IFIMAV, Spain; CIBERSAM, Spain
Andrei Szoke, Hôpital Henri-Mondor, France

Copyright: © 2014 Aas, Dazzan, Mondelli, Melle, Murray and Pariante. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Monica Aas, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, TOP Study Group, KG Jebsen Psychosis Research Centre, Institute of Clinical Medicine, Bygg 49, Ullevål Sykehus, PO Box 4956, Nydalen, 0424 Oslo, Norway e-mail: monica.aas@medisin.uio.no

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.